Mind & Society

, Volume 11, Issue 1, pp 3–13 | Cite as

Defining features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type 2 processing

Article

Abstract

Many critics of dual-process models have mistaken long lists of descriptive terms in the literature for a full-blown theory of necessarily co-occurring properties. These critiques have distracted attention from the cumulative progress being made in identifying the much smaller set of properties that truly do define Type 1 and Type 2 processing. Our view of the literature is that autonomous processing is the defining feature of Type 1 processing. Even more convincing is the converging evidence that the key feature of Type 2 processing is the ability to sustain the decoupling of secondary representations. The latter is a foundational cognitive requirement for hypothetical thinking.

Keywords

Dual process theory Cognitive decoupling Autonomy 

References

  1. Aron AR (2008) Progress in executive-function research: from tasks to functions to regions to networks. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:124–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bargh JA (1994) The four horsemen of automaticity: awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In: Wyer RS Jr, Srull TK (eds) Handbook of social cognition, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, New Jersey, pp 1–40Google Scholar
  3. Barrett HC, Kurzban R (2006) Modularity in cognition: framing the debate. Psychol Rev 113:628–647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Best JR, Miller PH, Jones LL (2009) Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Dev Rev 29:180–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carruthers P (2006) The architecture of the mind. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Conway ARA, Cowan N, Bunting MF, Therriault DJ, Minkoff SRB (2002) A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30:163–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cosmides L (1989) The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31:187–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Coyle TR (2003) A review of the worst performance rule: evidence, theory, and alternative hypotheses. Intelligence 31:567–587CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dawes RM (1976) Shallow psychology. In: Carroll JS, Payne JW (eds) Cognition and social behavior. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 3–11Google Scholar
  10. Deary IJ (2000) Looking down on human intelligence: from psychometrics to the brain. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennett DC (1984) Elbow room: the varieties of free will worth wanting. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  12. Dienes Z, Perner J (1999) A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behav Brain Sci 22:735–808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Engle RW (2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11:19–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Evans JStBT (2007) Hypothetical thinking: dual processes in reasoning and judgment. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Evans JStBT (2008) Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59:255–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Evans JStBT (2009) How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many? In: Evans J, Frankish K (eds) In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 33–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Evans JStBT (2010) Thinking twice: two minds in one brain. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Evans JStBT, Frankish K (2009) In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Evans JStBT, Over DE (1999) Explicit representations in hypothetical thinking. Behav Brain Sci 22:763–764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Evans JStBT, Over DE (2004) If. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  21. Evans JStBT, Over DE, Handley SJ (2003) A theory of hypothetical thinking. In: Hardman D, Maachi L (eds) Thinking: psychological perspectives on reasoning. Wiley, Chicester, pp 3–22Google Scholar
  22. Fodor JA (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  23. Gilbert DT (1999) What the mind’s not. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y (eds) Dual-process theories in social psychology. Guilford Press, New York, pp 3–11Google Scholar
  24. Hasher L, Lustig C, Zacks R (2007) Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In: Conway A, Jarrold C, Kane M, Miyake A, Towse J (eds) Variation in working memory. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 227–249Google Scholar
  25. Horn JL, Cattell RB (1967) Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychol 26:1–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ (2008) Improved fluid intelligence with training in working memory. P Natl A Sci USA 105:6829–6833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kahneman D (2003) A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol 58:697–720CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kahneman D, Frederick S (2002) Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (eds) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 49–81Google Scholar
  29. Kahneman D, Klein G (2009) Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. Am Psychol 64:515–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kane MJ, Engle RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon B Rev 9:637–671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Keren G, Schul Y (2009) Two is not always better than one: a critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspect Psychol Sci 4:533–550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kruglanski A, Gigerenzer G (2011) Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychol Rev 118:97–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. LaBerge D, Samuels S (1974) Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychol 6:293–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lepine R, Barrouillet P, Camos V (2005) What makes working memory spans so predictive of high-level cognition? Psychon B Rev 12:165–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Leslie AM (1987) Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychol Rev 94:412–426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lieberman MD (2003) Reflexive and reflective judgment processes: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. In: Forgas JP, Williams KR, von Hippel W (eds) Social judgments: implicit and explicit processes. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 44–67Google Scholar
  37. Lieberman MD (2007) Social cognitive neuroscience: areview of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol 58:259–289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Marcovitch S, Boseovski JJ, Knapp RJ, Kane MJ (2010) Goal neglect and working memory capacity in 4- to 6-year-old children. Child Dev 81:1687–1695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Newstead SE (2000) Are there two different types of thinking? Behav Brain Sci 23:690–691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nichols S, Stich SP (2003) Mindreading: an integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  41. Osman M (2004) An evaluation of dual-process theories of reasoning. Psychon B Rev 11:988–1010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Perner J (1991) Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  43. Posner MI, Snyder CRR (1975) Attention and cognitive control. In: Solso RL (ed) Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium. Wiley, New York, pp 55–85Google Scholar
  44. Salthouse TA, Pink JE (2008) Why is working memory related to fluid intelligence? Psychon B Rev 15:364–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Samuels R (2005) The complexity of cognition: tractability arguments for massive modularity. In: Carruthers P, Laurence S, Stich S (eds) The innate mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 107–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Samuels R (2009) The magical number two, plus or minus: dual-process theory as a theory of cognitive kinds. In: Evans JStBT, Frankish K (eds) In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 129–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychol Rev 84:127–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Slovic P, Peters E (2006) Risk perception and affect. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15:322–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sperber D (1994) The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In: Hirschfeld LA, Gelman SA (eds) Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 39–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sperber D (2000) Metarepresentations in evolutionary perspective. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 117–137Google Scholar
  51. Stanovich KE (1990) Concepts in developmental theories of reading skill: cognitive resources, automaticity, and modularity. Dev Rev 10:72–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stanovich KE (1999) Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Erlbaum, MahwahGoogle Scholar
  53. Stanovich KE (2004) The robot’s rebellion: finding meaning in the age of Darwin. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  54. Stanovich KE (2009) Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: is it time for a tri-process theory? In: Evans JStBT, Frankish K (eds) In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 55–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Stanovich KE (2011) Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  56. Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a hostile world: the evolution of human cognition. Blackwell Publishing, MaldenGoogle Scholar
  57. Taylor SE (1981) The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In: Harvey JH (ed) Cognition, social behavior, and the environment. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 189–211Google Scholar
  58. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185:1124–1131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wang L (2009) Money and fame: vividness effects in the National Basketball Association. J Behav Decis Making 22:20–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. West RF, Stanovich KE (1978) Automatic contextual facilitation in readers of three ages. Child Dev 49:717–727CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Zbrodoff NJ, Logan GD (1986) On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. J Exp Psychol Gen 115:118–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Zelazo PD (2004) The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends Cogn Sci 8:12–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Human Development and Applied PsychologyUniversity of TorontoTorontoCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyYork UniversityTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations