Mind & Society

, Volume 10, Issue 1, pp 57–78 | Cite as

Affective problem solving: emotion in research practice

Article

Abstract

This paper presents an analysis of emotional and affectively toned discourse in biomedical engineering researchers’ accounts of their problem solving practices. Drawing from our interviews with scientists in two laboratories, we examine three classes of expression: explicit, figurative and metaphorical, and attributions of emotion to objects and artifacts important to laboratory practice. We consider the overall function of expressions in the particular problem solving contexts described. We argue that affective processes are engaged in problem solving, not as simply tacked onto reasoning but as integral to it. The examples we present illustrate the close relation of emotion to problem solving and experimentation; they also implicate social and cultural dimensions of emotion expression. The analysis underscores a need to consider emotional expression to be intimately and importantly tied to the cognitive achievements and social negotiations of laboratory practices.

Keywords

Emotion Affect Cognition Science practice Motivation Metaphor Problem solving 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Science Foundation ROLE Grants REC0106773 and DRL0411825 to Nancy Nersessian and Wendy Newstetter in conducting the research on the laboratories. This analysis was completed while the first author was a senior research scientist on the latter. The data derive from ethnographic research conducted with the assistance of Wendy Newstetter and our research group, especially Ellie Harmon, Elke Kurz-Milcke, Christopher Patton, and Sanjay Chandrasekharan. We thank the members of the research labs for allowing us into their work environment, letting us observe them, and granting us numerous interviews.

References

  1. Arnold MB (1960) Emotion and personality. vol 1: psychological aspects. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown JS, Collins A, Duguid P (1989) Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educ Res 18:32–42Google Scholar
  3. Emirbayer M, Goldberg CA (2005) Pragmatism, bourdieu, and collective emotions in contentious politics. Theor Soc 34(5/6):469–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Feist GJ (2006) The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  5. Fetzer J (2003) The sky is not falling…: accepting failure to create innovation in experimentation. Ann Biol Chem 375(5):597–598Google Scholar
  6. Franken R (2006) Human motivation, 6th edn. Wadsworth, FlorenceGoogle Scholar
  7. Fridlund AJ (1994) Human facial expression: an evolutionary view. Academic Press, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  8. Fridlund AJ, Russell JA (2006) The functions of facial expressions: what’s in a face? In: Manusov V, Patterson ML (eds) Sage handbook of nonverbal communication. Sage, Newbury Park, pp 299–320Google Scholar
  9. Glaser B, Strauss A (1967) The discovery of grounded theory: strategies for qualitative research. Aldine Publishing Company, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  10. Greeno JG (1998) The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. Am Psychol 53:5–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Griffiths P, Scarantino A (2008) Emotions in the wild: the situated perspective on emotion. In: Robbins P, Aydede M (eds) The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 437–453Google Scholar
  12. Haidt J (2007) The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science 18(316):998–1002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hutchins E (1995a) How a cockpit remembers its speeds. Cogn Sci 19:265–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hutchins E (1995b) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  15. Johnson M (2008) The meaning of the body: aesthetics of human understanding. University Of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  16. Kövecses Z (2000) Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture and body in human feeling. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Kuhn T (1977) Objectivity, value judgment, and theory choice. In: Kuhn T (ed) The essential tension: selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 320–339Google Scholar
  18. Lakoff G, Johnson M (1999) Philosophy in the flesh. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Lazarus RS (2006) Stress and emotion: a new synthesis. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Lazarus RS, Folkman S (1987) Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping. Eur J Pers 1:141–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lazarus A, Averill JR, Opton EM (1970) Toward a cognitive theory of emotions. In: Arnold MB (ed) Feelings and emotions. Academic Press, New York, pp 207–232Google Scholar
  22. LeDoux J (1996) The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon and Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Mahoney MJ (1976) Scientist as subject. Ballinger Publishing, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  24. Maslow A (1966) The psychology of science: a reconnaissance. Harper & Row, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  25. McAllister JW (1996) Beauty and revolution in science. Cornell, IthicaGoogle Scholar
  26. Mitroff I (1974) The subjective side of science. Elsevier, AmstredamGoogle Scholar
  27. Moore S, Oaksford M (2002) Emotional cognition. John Benjamins, AmstredamGoogle Scholar
  28. Nersessian NJ (2009) How do engineering scientists think? Model-based simulation in biomedical engineering laboratories. Topics 1(3):730–757Google Scholar
  29. Nersessian NJ, Kurz-Milcke E, Newstetter WC, Davies J (2003) Research laboratories as evolving distributed cognitive systems. In: Proceedings of the 25th annual conference of the cognitive science society, pp 857–862Google Scholar
  30. Osbeck L, Nersessian N (2006) The distribution of representation. J Theor Soc Behav 36(2):141–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Parkinson B, Fischer AH, Manstead ASR (2005) Emotions in social relations: Cultural, group and interpersonal processes. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Petroski H (2008) Success through failure: the paradox of design. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  33. Polanyi M (1973) Personal knowledge: towards a post-critical philosophy. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Originally published 1958Google Scholar
  34. Resnick LB, Pontecorvo R, Säljö C (1997) Discourse, tools, and reasoning. In: Resnick LB, Pontecorvo R, Säljö C, Burge B (eds) Discourse, tools and reasoning: essays on situated cognition. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 1–22Google Scholar
  35. Roseman IJ (1991) Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cogn Emotion 5:161–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Simonton DK (2004) Creativity in science: chance, logic, genius, and zeitgeist. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  37. Strauss A, Corbin J (1998) Basics of qualitative research: techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory, 2nd edn. Thousand Oaks, SageGoogle Scholar
  38. Thagard P (2008) Hot thought. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  39. Tweney R (1989) A framework for cognitive psychology of science. In: Gholson B, Neimeyer RA, Houts AC (eds) Psychology of science: contributions to metascience. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 342–366Google Scholar
  40. Varela F, Thompson E, Rosch E (1991) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  41. Watson JD (1969) The double helix. New American Library, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of West GeorgiaCarrolltonUSA
  2. 2.School of Interactive ComputingGeorgia Institute of TechnologyAtlantaUSA

Personalised recommendations