Mind & Society

, Volume 7, Issue 1, pp 109–128

Language co-evolved with the rule of law

Original Article

Abstract

Many scholars assume a connection between the evolution of language and that of distinctively human group-level morality. Unfortunately, such thinkers frequently downplay a central implication of modern Darwinian theory, which precludes the possibility of innate psychological mechanisms evolving to benefit the group at the expense of the individual. Group level moral regulation is indeed central to public life in all known human communities. The production of speech acts would be impossible without this. The challenge, therefore, is to explain on a Darwinian basis how life could have become subject to the rule of law. Only then will we have an appropriate social framework in which to contextualize our models of how language may have evolved.

Keywords

Language evolution Human origins Ritual Religion Totemism Trust Primates Signal evolution Darwinism Menstruation Commitment Contracts Reliability 

References

  1. Aiello LC, Dunbar RIM (1993) Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Curr Anthropol 34:184–193Google Scholar
  2. Aiello LC, Wheeler P (1995) The expensive tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol 36:199–221Google Scholar
  3. Alcorta CS, Sosis R (2005) Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: the evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Hum Nat 16(4):323–359Google Scholar
  4. Atran S (2002) In gods we trust: the evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Austin JL (1978 [1955]) How to do things with words. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  6. Axelrod R (1986) An evolutionary approach to social norms. Am Polit Sci Rev 80:1095–1111Google Scholar
  7. Boehm C (2001) Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  8. Botha R (2003) Unravelling the evolution of language. Elsevier, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  9. Bourdieu P (1991) Language and symbolic power. Polity Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  10. Bowles S (2006) Group competition, reproductive levelling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314:1569–1572Google Scholar
  11. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizeable groups. Ethol Sociobiol 13:171–195Google Scholar
  13. Boyer P (2001) Religion explained. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Brody H (2000) The other side of Eden: hunters, farmers and the shaping of the world. North Point Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2003) Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature (Ldn) 143:297–299Google Scholar
  16. Brown P, Levinson SC (1987 [1978]) Politeness: some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Burling R (2005) The talking ape: how language evolved. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Chase P (1999) Symbolism as reference and symbolism as culture. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 34–49Google Scholar
  19. Chomsky N (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  20. Chomsky N (1988) Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  21. Chomsky N (1995) The minimalist programme. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  22. Cowlishaw G, Dunbar RIM (1991) Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Anim Behav 41:1045–1056Google Scholar
  23. Dawkins R (2006 [1976]) The selfish gene, 30th anniversary edn. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  24. Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1978) Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 282–309Google Scholar
  25. De Waal FBM (1996) Good natured: the origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  26. Dessalles JL (1998) Altruism, status and the origin of relevance. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 130–47Google Scholar
  27. Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems: studies in behavioural adaptation. Croom Helm, LondonGoogle Scholar
  28. Dunbar RIM (1999) Culture, honesty and the freerider problem. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture: an interdisciplinary view. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 194–213Google Scholar
  29. Durkheim E (1963 [1898]) La prohibition de l’inceste et ses origins. Année Sociol 1:1–70. (Reprinted as Incest: the nature and origin of the taboo, trans E Sagarin. Stuart, New York)Google Scholar
  30. Durkheim E (1965 [1912]) The elementary forms of the religious life. Free Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  31. Ellis L (1995) Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a cross-species comparison. Ethol Sociobiol 16:257–333Google Scholar
  32. Engels F (1972 [1884]) The origin of the family, private property and the state. Pathfinder Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  33. Enquist M, Leimar O (1993) The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Anim Behav 45:747–757Google Scholar
  34. Erdal D, Whiten A (1994) On human egalitarianism: an evolutionary product of Machiavellian status escalation? Curr Anthropol 35(2):175–183Google Scholar
  35. Erdal D, Whiten A (1996) Egalitarianism and Machiavellian intelligence in human evolution. In: Mellars P, Gibson K (eds) Modelling the early human mind. McDonald Institute Monographs, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  36. Flack J, Girvan M, de Waal FBM, Krakauer DC (2006) Policing stabilizes construction of social niches in primates. Nature (Ldn) 439:426–429Google Scholar
  37. Foley RA, Lee PC (1991) Ecology and energetics of encephalization in hominid evolution. Phil Trans R Soc Ldn 334:223–232Google Scholar
  38. Fouts RS (1975) Capacities for language in great apes. In: Tuttle RH (ed) Socioecology and psychology of primates. Mouton, The Hague, pp 371–390Google Scholar
  39. Fouts RS (1997) Next of kin: my conversations with chimpanzees. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  40. Furuichi T (1989) Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. Int J Primatol 10(3):173–197Google Scholar
  41. Gellner E (1988) Origins of society. In: Fabian AC (ed) Origins: the Darwin college lectures. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  42. Gintis H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. J Theor Biol 206:169–179Google Scholar
  43. Gintis H, Alden Smith E, Bowles S (2001) Costly signalling and cooperation. J Theor Biol 213:103–119Google Scholar
  44. Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2003) Explaining altruistic behaviour in humans. Evol Hum Behav 24:153–172Google Scholar
  45. Grafen A (1990a) Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher process. J Theor Biol 144:473–516Google Scholar
  46. Grafen A (1990b) Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol 144:517–546Google Scholar
  47. Grice HP (1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  48. Gürerk Ö, Irlenbusch B, Rockenbach B (2006) The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312:108–111Google Scholar
  49. Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour I, II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52Google Scholar
  50. Harris R (1988) Language, Saussure and Wittgenstein: how to play games with words. Routledge, London/New YorkGoogle Scholar
  51. Hauser MD (1992) Costs of deception: cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 82:12137–12139Google Scholar
  52. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579Google Scholar
  53. Hawkes K (1990a) Showing off: tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethol Sociobiol 12:29–54Google Scholar
  54. Hawkes K (1990b) Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In: Cashdan E (ed) Risk and uncertainty. Westview Press, Boulder Colarado, pp 145–166Google Scholar
  55. Hawkes K (1993) Why hunter–gatherers work: an ancient version of the problem of public goods. Curr Anthropol 34:341–361Google Scholar
  56. Hawkes K (1996) Foraging differences between men and women: behavioural ecology of the sexual division of labour. In: Steele J, Shennan S (eds) The archaeology of human ancestry: power, sex and tradition. Routledge, London, pp 283–305Google Scholar
  57. Henrich J, Boyd R (2001) Why people punish defectors: weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. J Theor Biol 208:79–89Google Scholar
  58. Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196Google Scholar
  59. Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas J, MGurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312:1768–1770Google Scholar
  60. Irons W (2001) Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In: Nesse R (ed) Evolution and the capacity for commitment. Russell Sage, New York, pp 292–309Google Scholar
  61. Key CA, Aiello LC (1999) The evolution of social organisation. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 15–33Google Scholar
  62. Key C, Aiello LC (2000) A prisoner’s dilemma model of the evolution of paternal care. Folia Primatol 71:77–92Google Scholar
  63. Knauft BB (1991) Violence and sociality in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 32:391–428Google Scholar
  64. Knight C (1991) Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture. Yale University Press, New Haven/LondonGoogle Scholar
  65. Knight C (2000) Play as precursor of phonology and syntax. In: Knight CD, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford JR (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 99–119Google Scholar
  66. Knight C, Power C (2005) Grandmothers, politics, and getting back to science. In: Voland E, Chasiotis A, Schienfenhövel W (eds) Grandmotherhood: the evolutionary significance of the second half of female life. Rutgers, New Brunswick, pp 81–98Google Scholar
  67. Knight C, Power C, Watts I (1995) The human symbolic revolution: a Darwinian account. Camb Archaeol J 5:75–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature (Ldn) 435:673–676Google Scholar
  69. Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 380–402Google Scholar
  70. Lee RB (1988) Reflections on primitive communism. In: Ingold T, Riches D, Woodburn J (eds) Hunters and gatherers, vol. I: history, evolution and social change. Aldine, Chicago, pp 252–268Google Scholar
  71. Leech G (1980) Language and tact. In: Explorations in semantics and pragmatics: pragmatics and beyond. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 79–117Google Scholar
  72. Lévi-Strauss C (1969) The elementary structures of kinship. Eyre and Spottiswoode, LondonGoogle Scholar
  73. Martin RD (1990) Primate origins and evolution. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar
  74. Maynard Smith J, Szathmáry E (1995) The major transitions in evolution. WH Freeman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  75. Mellars PA, Stringer C (1989) The human revolution: behavioural and biological perspective in the origins of modern humans. Edinburgh University Press, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  76. Morgan LH (1871) Systems of consanguinity and affinity of the human family. Smithsonian Institution, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
  77. Newmeyer FJ (2003) Review article. Language 79(3):583–599Google Scholar
  78. Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature (Ldn) 437:1291–1298Google Scholar
  79. O’Connell JF, Hawkes K, Blurton Jones N (1999) Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. J Hum Evol 36:461–485Google Scholar
  80. O’Connell JF, Hawkes K, Lupo KD, Blurton Jones NG (2002) Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. J Hum Evol 43:831–872Google Scholar
  81. Parish A, de Waal FBM (2000) The other ‘closest living relative’: how Bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution. Ann N Y Acad Sci 907:97–113Google Scholar
  82. Pepperberg I (2005) An avian perspective on language evolution: implications of simultaneous development of vocal and physical object combinations by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). In: Tallerman M (ed) Language origins: perspectives on evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 239–261Google Scholar
  83. Pinker S, Jackendoff R (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition 95:201–236Google Scholar
  84. Power C (2000) Secret language use at female initiation: bounding gossiping communities. In: Knight C, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford JR (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 81–98Google Scholar
  85. Power C, Aiello LC (1997) Female proto-symbolic strategies. In: Hager LD (ed) Women in human evolution. Routledge, New York/London, pp 153–171Google Scholar
  86. Power C, Watts I (1997) The woman with the zebra’s penis: gender, mutability and performance. J R Anthrop Inst 3(NS):537–560Google Scholar
  87. Price M, Cosmides L, Tooby J (2002) Punitive sentiment as an anti-free rider psychological device. Evol Hum Behav 23:203–231Google Scholar
  88. Rappaport RA (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  89. Rousseau JJ (1973 [1762]) The social contract. In: Rousseau JJ (ed) The social contract and discourses, trans GDH Cole. Dent, London/Melbourne, pp 179–309Google Scholar
  90. Runciman WG (2001) From nature to culture, from culture to society. In: Runciman WG (ed) The origin of social institutions. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 235–254Google Scholar
  91. Sahlins MD (1960) The origin of society. Sci Am 203(3):76–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. de Saussure F (1974 [1915]) Course in general linguistics, trans W Baskin. Fontana/Collins, LondonGoogle Scholar
  93. Savage-Rumbaugh S, Shanker G, Taylor TJ (1998) Apes, language and the human mind. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  94. Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication. Princeton University Press, Princeton/OxfordGoogle Scholar
  95. Searle JR (1971) The philosophy of language. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  96. Searle JR (1996) The construction of social reality. Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  97. Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Mascaro J, Schapiro SJ (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature (Ldn) 437:1357–1359Google Scholar
  98. Sperber D, Wilson D (1986) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  99. Stompka P (1999) Trust. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  100. Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  101. Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35:137–154Google Scholar
  102. Tooby J, DeVore I (1987) The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modelling. In: Kinzey WG (ed) The evolution of human behavior: primate models. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 183–237Google Scholar
  103. Trager GL (1972) Language and languages. Chandler, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  104. Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57Google Scholar
  105. Trivers RL (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool 14:249–264Google Scholar
  106. Ulbaek I (1998) The origin of language and cognition. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 30–43Google Scholar
  107. Van Gennep A (1960 [1909]) The rites of passage. Routledge & Kegan Paul, LondonGoogle Scholar
  108. Watts I (1999) The origin of symbolic culture. In: Dunbar R, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 113–146Google Scholar
  109. Watts I (2005) ‘Time, too, grows on the Moon’: some evidence for Knight’s theory of a human universal. In: James W, Mills D (eds) The qualities of time: anthropological approaches. Berg, New York, pp 95–118Google Scholar
  110. Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection: a critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton University Press, PrincetoneGoogle Scholar
  111. Wittgenstein L (1968) Philosophical investigations, trans Anscombe GEM. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  112. Zahavi A (1993) The fallacy of conventional signalling. Phil Trans R Soc Ldn 340:227–230Google Scholar
  113. Zahavi A (2003) Indirect selection and individual selection in sociobiology: my personal views on theories of social behaviour. Anim Behav 65:859–863Google Scholar
  114. Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle: a missing piece in Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press, Oxford/New YorkGoogle Scholar
  115. Zak PJ, Knack S (2001) Trust and growth. Econ J 111:295–321Google Scholar
  116. Zuckerman S (1932) The social life of monkeys and apes. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner, LondonGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Fondazione Rosselli 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Social Sciences, Media and Cultural StudiesUniversity of East LondonLondonEngland

Personalised recommendations