Advertisement

Journal of Experimental Criminology

, Volume 10, Issue 4, pp 459–486 | Cite as

Accusatorial and information-gathering interrogation methods and their effects on true and false confessions: a meta-analytic review

  • Christian A. Meissner
  • Allison D. Redlich
  • Stephen W. Michael
  • Jacqueline R. Evans
  • Catherine R. Camilletti
  • Sujeeta Bhatt
  • Susan Brandon
Article

Abstract

Objectives

We completed a systematic review and meta-analysis of the available empirical literature assessing the influence of accusatorial and information-gathering methods of interrogation in eliciting true and false confessions.

Methods

We conducted two separate meta-analyses. The first meta-analysis focused on observational field studies that assessed the association between certain interrogation methods and elicitation of a confession statement. The second meta-analysis focused on experimental, laboratory-based studies in which ground truth was known (i.e., a confession is factually true or false). We located 5 field studies and 12 experimental studies eligible for the meta-analyses. We coded outcomes from both study types and report mean effect sizes with 95 % confidence intervals. A random effects model was used for analysis of effect sizes. Moderator analyses were conducted when appropriate.

Results

Field studies revealed that both information-gathering and accusatorial approaches were more likely to elicit a confession when compared with direct questioning methods. However, experimental studies revealed that the information-gathering approach preserved, and in some cases increased, the likelihood of true confessions, while simultaneously reducing the likelihood of false confessions. In contrast, the accusatorial approach increased both true and false confessions when compared with a direct questioning method.

Conclusions

The available data support the effectiveness of an information-gathering style of interviewing suspects. Caution is warranted, however, due to the small number of independent samples available for the analysis of both field and experimental studies. Additional research, including the use of quasi-experimental field studies, appears warranted.

Keywords

Interrogation Interview Confession Police 

References

* Studies included in the meta-analysis are denoted with an asterisk.

  1. Abboud, B., Wadkins, T. A., Forrest, K. D., Lange, J., & Alavi, S. (2002, March). False confessions: Is the gender of the interrogator a determining factor? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
  2. Baldwin, J. (1992). Videotaping police interviews with suspects: A national evaluation. Police Research Series Paper 1. London: Home Office.Google Scholar
  3. Baldwin, J. (1993). Police interview techniques: establishing truth or proof? British Journal of Criminology, 33, 325–352.Google Scholar
  4. Behrman, B. W., & Davey, S. L. (2001). Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: An archival analysis. Law & Human Behavior, 25, 475–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beune, K. (2009). Talking heads: Interviewing suspects from a cultural perspective. Doctoral dissertation, University of Twente.Google Scholar
  6. Beune, K., Giebels, E., & Sanders, K. (2009). Are you talking to me? Influencing behavior and culture in police interviews. Psychology, Crime & Law, 15, 597–617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. * Billings, F. J., Taylor, T., Burns, J., Corey, D. L., Garven, S., & Wood, J. M. (2007). Can reinforcement induce children to falsely incriminate themselves? Law & Human Behavior, 31, 125–139.Google Scholar
  8. * Blair, J. P. (2007). The roles of interrogation, perception, and individual differences in producing compliant false confessions. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 13, 173–186.Google Scholar
  9. Bull, R., & Soukara, S. (2010). What really happens in police interviews. In G. D. Lassiter & C. A. Meissner (Eds.), Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  10. Bull, R., Valentine, T., & Williamson, T. (2009). Handbook of psychology of investigative interviewing. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cassell, P. G., & Hyman, B. S. (1996). Dialogue on Miranda: police interrogation in the 1990s: an empirical study of the effects of Miranda. UCLA Law Review, 43, 839.Google Scholar
  12. * Cole, T., Teboul, J. C., Zulawski, D., Wicklander, D., & Sturman, S. (2005). Trying to obtain false confessions through the use of false evidence: A replication of Kassin and Kiechel’s study. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, New York, NY. Retrieved December 19, 2008. http://www.allacademic.com/meta/p13137_index.html.
  13. Costanzo, M. & Redlich, A. (2010). Use of physical and psychological force in criminal and military interrogations. In J. Knuttsson, & J. Kuhns (Eds.), Policing around the world: Police use of force. Santa Barbara: Praeger Security International.Google Scholar
  14. Cox, D. R. (1970). Analysis of binary data. New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
  15. DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. DesLauriers-Varin, N., Lussier, P., & St-Yves, M. (2011). Confessing their crime: factors influencing the offender’s decision to confess to the police. Justice Quarterly, 28, 113–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dixon, D. (2007). Interrogating images: Audio-visually recorded police questioning of suspects. Sydney: Sydney Institute of Criminology.Google Scholar
  18. Evans, J. R., Meissner, C. A., Brandon, S. E., Russano, M. B., & Kleinman, S. M. (2010). Criminal versus HUMINT interrogations: the importance of psychological science to improving interrogative practice. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 38, 215–249.Google Scholar
  19. Evans, J. R., Meissner, C. A., Ross, A. B., Houston, K. A., Russano, M. B., & Horgan, A. J. (2013). Obtaining guilty knowledge in human intelligence interrogations: comparing accusatorial and information-gathering approaches with a novel experimental paradigm. Journal of Applied Research in Memory & Cognition, 2, 83–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Faller, K. C., Birdsall, W. C., Henry, J., Vandervort, F., & Silverschanz, P. (2001). What makes sex offender confess? An exploratory study. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 10, 31–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fein, R. (2006). Educing information. Interrogation: Science and art. Intelligence science board, phase 1 report. Washington, DC: National Defense Intelligence College.Google Scholar
  22. Feld, B. C. (2006). Police interrogation of juveniles: an empirical study of policy and practice. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 97, 219–316.Google Scholar
  23. Feld, B. C. (2013). Kids, cops, and confessions: Inside the interrogation room. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Fisher, R. P., & Geiselman, R. E. (1992). Memory enhancing techniques for investigative interviewing: The cognitive interview. Springfield: Charles C Thomas.Google Scholar
  25. Fisher, R. P., & Perez, V. (2007). Memory-enhancing techniques for interviewing crime suspects. In S. Christianson (Ed.), Offenders’ memories of violent crimes (pp. 329–350). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Google Scholar
  26. Forrest, K. D., Wadkins, T. A., & Larson, B. A. (2006). Suspect personality, police interrogations, and false confessions: maybe it is not just the situation. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 621–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Griffiths, A. (2008). An examination into the efficacy of police advanced investigative interview training. Doctoral dissertation, University of Portsmouth.Google Scholar
  28. Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  29. Hartwig, M., Meissner, C. A., & Semmel, M. D. (2014). Human intelligence interviewing and interrogation: Assessing the challenges of developing an ethical, evidence-based approach. In R. Bull (Ed.), Investigative interviewing (pp. 209–228). Springer.Google Scholar
  30. * Hill, C., Memon, A., & McGeorge, P. (2008). The role of confirmation bias in suspect interviews: A systematic evaluation. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 357–371.Google Scholar
  31. Home Office. (2003). Police and criminal evidence act 1984. Codes of practice A-E revised edition. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
  32. Horgan, A. J., Russano, M. B., Meissner, C. A., & Evans, J. (2012). Minimization and maximization techniques: assessing the perceived consequences of confessing and confession diagnosticity. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18, 65–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Horselenberg, R., Merckelbach, H., & Josephs, S. (2003). Individual differences and false confessions: a conceptual replication of Kassin and Kiechel. Psychology, Crime & Law, 9, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2001). Criminal interrogation and confessions (4th ed.). Gaithersberg: Aspen.Google Scholar
  35. Justice, B. P., Bhatt, S., Brandon, S. E., & Kleinman, S. M. (2009). Army field manual 2–22.3 interrogation methods: A science-based review. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  36. Kassin, S. M., Goldstein, C. C., & Savitsky, K. (2003). Behavioral confirmation in the interrogation room: On the dangers of presuming guilt. Law & Human Behavior, 27, 187–203.Google Scholar
  37. Kassin, S. M., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (2004). The psychology of confessions: a review of the literature and issues. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 33–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. * Kassin, S. M., & Kiechel, K. L. (1996). The social psychology of false confessions: compliance, internalization, and confabulation. Psychological Science, 7, 125–128.Google Scholar
  39. Kassin, S. M., Leo, R. A., Meissner, C. A., Richman, K. D., Colwell, L. H., Leach, A.-M., et al. (2007). Police interviewing and interrogation: a self-report survey of police practices and beliefs. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 381–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kassin, S. M., Drizin, S. A., Grisso, T., Gudjonsson, G. H., Leo, R. A., & Redlich, A. D. (2010). Police-induced confessions: risk factors and recommendations. Law and Human Behavior, 34, 3–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kebbell, M. R., & Daniels, T. (2006). Mock-suspects’ decisions to confess: the influence of eyewitness statements and identifications. Psychiatry, Psychology, and Law, 13, 261–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kebbell, M. R., Hurren, E. J., & Roberts, S. (2006). Mock-suspects’ decisions to confess: the accuracy of eyewitness evidence is critical. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 477–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. * King, L. & Snook, B. (2009). Peering inside a Canadian interrogation room: an examination of the Reid model of interrogation, influence tactics, and coercive strategies. Criminal Justice & Behavior, 36, 674–694.Google Scholar
  44. Klaver, J., Lee, Z., & Rose, V. G. (2008). Effects of personality, interrogation techniques, and plausibility in an experimental false confession paradigm. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 71–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lassiter, G. D., & Meissner, C. A. (2010). Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations. Washington, DC: APA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. * Leo, R. A. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 86, 266–303.Google Scholar
  47. Leo, R. A. (2008). Police interrogation and American justice. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Leo, R. A., & Ofshe, R. (1998). The consequences of false confessions: deprivations of liberty and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological interrogation. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 88, 429–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lippert, T., Cross, T. P., Jones, L. M., & Walsh, W. (2010). Suspect confession of child sexual abuse to investigators. Child Maltreatment, 15, 161–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  51. Ma, Y. (2007). A comparative view of the law of interrogation. International Criminal Justice Review, 17, 5–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. McConville, M., & Baldwin, J. (1982). The role of interrogation in crime discovery and conviction. British Journal of Criminology, 22, 165–175.Google Scholar
  53. McGurk, B., Carr, M., & McGurk, D. (1993). Investigative interviewing courses for police officers: An evaluation. Police Research Group Paper 4. London: Home Office.Google Scholar
  54. Medalie, R. J., Zeitz, & Alexander. (1968). Custodial police interrogation in our nation’s capital: the attempt to implement Miranda. Michigan Law Review, 66, 1347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Medford, S., Gudjonsson, G. H., & Pearse, J. (2003). The efficacy of the appropriate adult safeguard during police interviewing. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 8, 253–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Meissner, C. A., & Kassin, S. M. (2002). “He’s guilty!”: investigator bias in judgments of truth and deception. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 469–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Meissner, C. A., & Kassin, S. M. (2004). “You’re guilty, so just confess!”: Cognitive and behavioral confirmation biases in the interrogation room. In D. Lassiter’s (Ed.), Interrogations, confessions, and entrapment (pp. 85–106). Kluwer Academic / Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  58. Meissner, C. A., Hartwig, M., & Russano, M. B. (2010a). The need for a positive psychological approach and collaborative effort for improving practice in the interrogation room. Law and Human Behavior, 34, 43–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Meissner, C. A., Russano, M. B., & Narchet, F. M. (2010b). The importance of a laboratory science for improving the diagnostic value of confession evidence. In G. D. Lassiter & C. Meissner’s (Eds.), Interrogations and confessions: Research, practice, and policy. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
  60. * Meissner, C. A., Russano, M. B., Rigoni, M. E., & Horgan, A. J. (2011). Is it time for a revolution in the interrogation room? Empirically validating information-gathering methods. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  61. Memon, A., Meissner, C. A., & Faser, J. (2010). The Cognitive Interview: a meta-analytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16, 340–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Milne, R., & Bull, R. (1999). Investigative interviewing: Psychology and practice. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  63. Mortimer, A., & Shepherd, E. (1999). Frames of mind: Schemata guiding cognition and conduct in the interviewing of suspected offenders. In A. Memon & R. Bull (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of interviewing (pp. 293–315). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  64. Moston, S., Stephenson, G. M., & Williamson, T. M. (1992). The effects of case characteristics on suspect behaviour during questioning. British Journal of Criminology, 32, 23–40.Google Scholar
  65. * Narchet, F., Meissner, C. A., & Russano, M. B. (2011). Modeling the influence of investigator bias on the elicitation of true and false confessions. Law and Human Behavior, 35, 452–465.Google Scholar
  66. Nash, R. A., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Innocent but proven guilty: eliciting internalized false confessions using doctored-video evidence. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 624–637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Neubauer, D. W. (1974). Confessions in Prairie City: some causes and effects. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 65, 103–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. New Haven Study. (1967). Interrogations in New Haven: the impact of Miranda. Yale Law Journal, 76, 1519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. * Newring, K. A. B., & O'Donohue, W. (2008). False confessions and influenced witness statements. Applied Psychology in Criminal Justice, 4, 81–107.Google Scholar
  70. Pearse, J. J. (2009). The investigation of terrorist offences in the United Kingdom: The context and climate for interviewing officers. In R. Bull, T. Valentine, & T. Williamson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology of investigative interviewing (pp. 69–90). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Pearse, J., & Gudjonsson, G. (1999). Measuring influential police interviewing tactics: a factor analytic approach. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 4, 221–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. * Pearse, J., Gudjonsson, G. H., Clare, I. C. H., & Rutter, S. (1998). Police interviewing and psychological vulnerabilities: predicting the likelihood of a confession. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 8, 1–21.Google Scholar
  73. * Perillo, J. T. & Kassin, S. M. (2011). Inside interrogation: the lie, the bluff, and false confessions. Law and Human Behavior, 35, 327337.Google Scholar
  74. Redlich, A. D. (2007). Military versus police interrogations: similarities and differences. Peace & Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 13, 423–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. * Redlich, A. D., & Goodman, G. S. (2003). Taking responsibility for an act not committed: the influence of age and suggestibility. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 141156.Google Scholar
  76. Redlich, A. D., & Meissner, C. A. (2009). Techniques and controversies on the interrogation of suspects: The artful practice versus the scientific study. In J. Skeem, K. Douglas, & S. Lilienfeld (Eds.), Psychological science in the courtroom: Controversies and consensus (pp. 124–148). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  77. Reiss, A. J., & Black, D. J. (1967). Interrogation and the criminal process. The Annals of the American Academy of Political & Social Science, 1, 47–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. * Russano, M. B., Meissner, C. A., Narchet, F. M., & Kassin, S. M. (2005a). Investigating true and false confessions within a novel experimental paradigm. Psychological Science, 16, 481–486.Google Scholar
  79. * Russano, M. B., Narchet, F., & Meissner, C. A. (March, 2005b). Investigating the effects of presenting false evidence on true and false confessions. Poster presented at the 2005 American Psychology-Law Society Conference, La Jolla, CA.Google Scholar
  80. Sánchez-Meca, J., Marín-Martínez, F., & Chacón-Moscoso, S. (2003). Effect-size indices for dichotomized outcomes in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 8(4), 448–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Schollum, M. (2005). Investigative interviewing: The literature. New Zealand Police Department. Retrieved January 15, 2006. http://www.police.govt.nz/resources/2005/investigative-interviewing/investigative-interviewing.pdf.
  82. Seeburger, R. H., & Wettick, S. (1967). Miranda in Pittsburgh: a statistical study. University of Pittsburgh Law Review, 29, 1.Google Scholar
  83. Smith, S. M., Stinson, V., & Patry, M. W. (2009). Using the ‘Mr. Big’ technique to elicit confessions: Successful innovation or dangerous development in the Canadian legal system. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 15, 168–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Softley, P. (1980). Police interrogation: An observational study in four police stations. Home Office Research Study No. 61.Google Scholar
  85. * Soukara, S., Bull, R., Vrij, A., Turner, M., & Cherryman, J. (2009). What really happens in police interviews with suspects?: tactics and confessions. Psychology, Crime and Law, 15, 493–506.Google Scholar
  86. Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2006). Paraverbal indicators of deception: a meta-analytic synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 421–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2007). Moderators of nonverbal indicators of deception: a meta-analytic synthesis. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 13, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Stephenson, G., & Moston, S. (1994). Police interrogation. Psychology, Crime & Law, 1, 151–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. van Bergen, S., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2008). Interrogation techniques and memory distrust. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14, 425–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Vera Institute of Justice. (1967). Monitored interrogations project final report. Unpublished document.Google Scholar
  91. Vrij, A., Mann, S. A., Fisher, R. P., Leal, S., Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: the benefit of recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 253–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. * Walsh, D. W. & Bull, R. H. (2010). What really is effective in interviews with suspects? A study comparing interviewing skills against interviewing outcomes. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15¸ 305–321.Google Scholar
  93. Walsh, D. W., & Milne, R. (2008). Keeping the PEACE? A study of investigative interviewing practices in the public sector. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 39–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Williamson, T. (2006). Investigative interviewing: Rights, research, and regulation. Devon: Willan.Google Scholar
  95. Willis, C.F., Macleod, J. & Naish, P. (1988). The tape recording of police interview with suspects: a second interim report. Home Office Research Study No. 97. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
  96. Witt, J. (1973). Non-coercive interrogation and the administration of criminal justice: the impact of Miranda on police effectuality. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 64, 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christian A. Meissner
    • 1
  • Allison D. Redlich
    • 2
  • Stephen W. Michael
    • 3
  • Jacqueline R. Evans
    • 4
  • Catherine R. Camilletti
    • 5
  • Sujeeta Bhatt
    • 6
  • Susan Brandon
    • 6
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIowa State UniversityAmesUSA
  2. 2.University at Albany, SUNYAlbanyUSA
  3. 3.Mercer UniversityMaconUSA
  4. 4.Florida International UniversityUniversity ParkUSA
  5. 5.University of Texas at El PasoEl PasoUSA
  6. 6.U.S. Department of DefenseArlington CountyUSA

Personalised recommendations