Advertisement

Instructional Science

, Volume 45, Issue 2, pp 157–175 | Cite as

Stereotype threat and gender differences in chemistry

  • Cijy Elizabeth Sunny
  • Gita TaasoobshiraziEmail author
  • Lauren Clark
  • Gwen Marchand
Article

Abstract

Stereotype threat theory (STT) offers one explanation for achievement differences in math and science for both women and minority students. Specifically, STT posits that the perceived risk of confirming a negative stereotype about an individual’s identity group acts as a psychological burden that negatively impacts performance. This study examined the impact of stereotype threat (ST) on gender differences in chemistry achievement, self-efficacy, and test-anxiety using a four-group, quasi-experimental design. 153 introductory-level college chemistry students were randomly assigned to one of four ST conditions including an explicit ST condition, an implicit ST condition, a reverse ST condition, and a nullified condition. Results indicated that there were no gender differences by ST condition; however, overall, the men had higher self-efficacy and lower test-anxiety than the women. An analysis of open-ended questions asking students about their intent to major in chemistry, beliefs regarding barriers to their achievement on the chemistry test, and gender differences in opportunities and mental capacity to achieve in chemistry provided insight into the quantitative results. Implications of our findings for future research on ST are discussed.

Keywords

Stereotype threat Chemistry Gender differences Self-efficacy Test-anxiety 

References

  1. Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(2), 113–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 29–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bell, A. E., Spencer, S. J., Iserman, E., & Logel, C. E. (2003). Stereotype threat and women’s performance in engineering. Journal of Engineering Education, 92(4), 307–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blickenstaff, C. J. (2005). Women and science careers: leaky pipeline or gender filter? Gender and Education, 17(4), 369–386. doi: 10.1080/09540250500145072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brodish, A. B., & Devine, P. G. (2009). The role of performance–avoidance goals and worry in mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 180–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cavallo, A. M., Potter, W. H., & Rozman, M. (2004). Gender differences in learning constructs, shifts in learning constructs, and their relationship to course achievement in a structured inquiry, yearlong college physics course for life science majors. School Science and Mathematics, 104(6), 288–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chen, X. & Soldner, M. (2013). STEM Attrition: College Students’ Paths into and out of STEM Fields. Statistical Analysis Report. NCES 2014-001. National Center for Education Statistics.Google Scholar
  8. Cho, J. Y., & Lee, E. H. (2014). Reducing confusion about grounded theory and qualitative content analysis: Similarities and differences. The Qualitative Report, 19(32), 1–20.Google Scholar
  9. Deemer, E. D., Smith, J. L., Carroll, A. N., & Carpenter, J. P. (2014a). Academic procrastination in STEM: Interactive effects of stereotype threat and achievement goals. The Career Development Quarterly, 62(2), 143–155. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-0045.2014.00076.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Deemer, E. D., Thoman, D. B., Chase, J. P., & Smith, J. L. (2014b). Feeling the threat stereotype threat as a contextual barrier to women’s science career choice intentions. Journal of Career Development, 41(2), 141–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Desouza, J. M. S., & Czemiak, C. M. (2002). Social implications and gender differences among preschoolers: Implications for science activities. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 16, 175–188. doi: 10.1080/02568540209594983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Doan, L. & Hilpert, J. (2009). A Qualitative Investigation of Stereotype Threat Activation and Mediation. Presented at American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting, San Diego, CAGoogle Scholar
  13. Elo, S., & Kyngäs, H. (2008). The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 62(1), 107–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Enman, M., & Lupart, J. (2000). Talented female students’ resistance to science: An exploratory study of post-secondary achievement motivation, persistence, and epistemological characteristics. High Ability Studies, 11(2), 161–178. doi: 10.1080/13598130020001205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Flore, P. C., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 53(1), 25–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.Google Scholar
  18. Glynn, S. M., Taasoobshirazi, G., & Brickman, P. (2007). Nonscience majors learning science: A theoretical model of motivation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44, 1088–1107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Glynn, S. M., Taasoobshirazi, G., & Brickman, P. (2009). Science motivation questionnaire: Construct validation with nonscience majors. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46(2), 127–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 645–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Good, J. J., Woodzicka, J. A., & Wingfield, L. C. (2010). The effects of gender stereotypic and counter-stereotypic textbook images on science performance. The Journal of Social Psychology, 150(2), 132–147. doi: 10.1080/00224540903366552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goulding, C. (2002). Grounded theory: A practical guide for management, business and market researchers. Newbury Park: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Graneheim, U. H., & Lundman, B. (2004). Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Education Today, 24(2), 105–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Greene, B. A., & DeBacker, T. K. (2004). Goal and orientations toward the future: Links to motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 91–120. doi: 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000026608.50611.b4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Heyworth, R. M. (1999). Procedural and conceptual knowledge of expert and novice students for the solving of a basic problem in chemistry. International Journal of Science Education, 21(2), 195–211. doi: 10.1080/095006999290787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hill, C., Corbett, C., & St Rose, A. (2010). Why so few? Women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Washington, DC: American Association of University Women.Google Scholar
  27. Johns, M., Schmader, T., & Martens, A. (2005). Knowing is half the battle teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women’s math performance. Psychological Science, 16(3), 175–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Johnson, H. J., Barnard-Brak, L., Saxon, T. F., & Johnson, M. K. (2012). An experimental study of the effects of stereotype threat and stereotype lift on men and women’s performance in mathematics. The Journal of Experimental Education, 80(2), 137–149. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2011.567312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Keller, J. (2007). Stereotype threat in classroom settings: The interactive effect of domain identification, task difficulty and stereotype threat on female students’ maths performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(2), 323–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereotypes and women’s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women’s susceptibility to stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(5), 825–832. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Krippendorff, K. (2004). Reliability in content analysis: Some common misconceptions and recommendations. Human Communication Research, 30(3), 411–433.Google Scholar
  32. MacQueen, K. M., McLellan, E., Kay, K., & Milstein, B. (1998). Codebook development for team-based qualitative analysis. Cultural Anthropology Methods, 10(2), 31–36.Google Scholar
  33. Marchand, G.C. (2015). Stereotyp threat and female performance in secondary physics courses. Presented at the American Association of Physics Teachers, San Diego, CAGoogle Scholar
  34. Marchand, G. C., & Taasoobshirazi, G. (2013). Stereotype threat and women’s performance in physics. International Journal of Science Education, 35(18), 3050–3061. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2012.683461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Matson, J. (2013, April). Women are earning greater share of STEM degrees, but doctorates remain gender skewed. Scientific American, 308(5). Retrieved from http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/women-earning-greater-share-stem-degrees-doctorates-remain-gender-skewed/
  36. Mattern, N., & Schau, C. (2002). Gender differences in science attitude-achievement relationships over time among white middle-school students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(4), 324–340. doi: 10.1002/tea.10024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mayring, P. (2000). Qualitative content analysis. forum qualitative sozialforschung/forum: Qualitative social research, 1(2). http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0114-fqs0002204.
  38. McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(1), 83–90. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T. A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. National Science Foundation. (2015). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering. Retrieved from NSF website: http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/2015/nsf15311/digest/theme4.cfm#minoritybachelors
  41. Nguyen, H. H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. O’Brien, L. T., & Crandall, C. S. (2003). Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s math performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(6), 782–789. doi: 10.1177/0146167203252810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Olson, S., & Riordan, D. G. (2012). Engage to excel: Producing one million additional college graduates with degrees in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Executive Office of the President: Report to the President.Google Scholar
  44. Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., García, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women’s generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 55–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reger, D. L., Goode, R. S., & Mercer, E. E. (1997). Chemistry: Principles and practice. Belmont, CA: Brooks Cole.Google Scholar
  47. Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(2), 194–201. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schreier, M. (2012). Qualitative content analysis in practice. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  50. Shapiro, J. R. (2011). Different groups, different threats: A multi-threat approach to the experience of stereotype threats. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 464–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. She, H. C. (2001). Different gender students’ participation in the high-and low-achieving middle school questioning-orientated biology classrooms in Taiwan. Research in Science & Technological Education, 19(2), 147–158. doi: 10.1080/02635140120087696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Shin, N., & McGee, S. (2002, November). The influence of inquiry-based multimedia learning environment on specific problem-solving skills among ninth grade students across gender differences. Paper presented at Association for Educational Communications and Technology, Dallas, TX.Google Scholar
  53. Smith, J. L. (2004). Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of mediational variables and new performance goal directions. Educational Psychology Review, 16(3), 177–206. doi: 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000034020.20317.89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, 47(3–4), 179–191. doi: 10.1023/A:1021051223441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 415–437.Google Scholar
  56. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4–28. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Steinberg, J. R., Okun, M. A., & Aiken, L. S. (2012). Calculus GPA and math identification as moderators of stereotype threat in highly persistent women. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34(6), 534–543. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2012.727319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Stieff, M., Ryu, M., Dixon, B., & Hegarty, M. (2012). The role of spatial ability and strategy preference for spatial problem solving in organic chemistry. Journal of Chemical Education, 89(7), 854–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on black and white athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1213–1227. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent–child conversations about science: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 34–47. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Vollmeyer, R., Püttmann, A., & Imhof, M. (2009). How to improve women’s performance in physics through stereotype threat. In Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1471–1476).Google Scholar
  64. Wang, X. (2013). Modeling entrance into STEM fields of study among students beginning at community colleges and four-year institutions. Research in Higher Education, 54(6), 664–692. doi: 10.1007/s11162-013-9291-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Zumdahl, S. S. (1997). Chemistry. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cijy Elizabeth Sunny
    • 1
  • Gita Taasoobshirazi
    • 1
    Email author
  • Lauren Clark
    • 2
  • Gwen Marchand
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Quantitative and Mixed Research MethodologiesUniversity of CincinnatiCincinnatiUSA
  2. 2.Department of Developmental and Learning SciencesUniversity of CincinnatiCincinnatiUSA
  3. 3.Department of Educational Psychology & Higher EducationUniversity of Nevada, Las VegasLas VegasUSA

Personalised recommendations