Instructional Science

, Volume 40, Issue 4, pp 691–710 | Cite as

Evaluating metacognitive scaffolding in Guided Invention Activities

  • Ido Roll
  • Natasha G. Holmes
  • James Day
  • Doug Bonn
Article

Abstract

Invention and Productive Failure activities ask students to generate methods that capture the important properties of some given data (e.g., uncertainty) before being taught the expert solution. Invention and Productive Failure activities are a class of scientific inquiry activities in that students create, implement, and evaluate mathematical models based on data. Yet, lacking sufficient inquiry skills, students often do not actualize the full potential of these activities. We identified key invention strategies in which students often fail to engage: exploratory analysis, peer interaction, self-explanation, and evaluation. A classroom study with 134 students evaluated the effect of supporting these skills on the quality and outcomes of the invention process. Students in the Unguided Invention condition received conventional Invention Activities; students in the Guided Invention condition received complementary metacognitive scaffolding. Students were asked to invent methods for calculating uncertainties in best-fitting lines. Guided Invention students invented methods that included more conceptual features and ranked the given datasets more accurately, although the quality of their mathematical expressions was not improved. At the process level, Guided Invention students revised their methods more frequently and had more and better instances of unprompted self-explanations even on components of the activity that were not supported by the metacognitive scaffolding. Classroom observations are used to demonstrate the effect of the scaffolding on students’ learning behaviours. These results suggest that process guidance in the form of metacognitive scaffolding augments the inherent benefits of Invention Activities and can lead to gains at both domain and inquiry levels.

Keywords

Invention Activities Productive Failure Metacognitive scaffolding Inquiry learning Guided discovery 

References

  1. Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Azevedo, R., Cromley, J. G., & Seibert, D. (2004). Does adaptive scaffolding facilitate students’ ability to regulate their learning with hypermedia? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 344–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Azevedo, R., & Jacobson, M. J. (2008). Advances in scaffolding learning with hypertext and hypermedia: A summary and critical analysis. Educational Technology Research and Development, 56, 93–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bulu, S., & Pedersen, S. (2010). Scaffolding middle school students’ content knowledge and ill-structured problem solving in a problem-based hypermedia learning environment. Educational Technology Research and Development, 58(5), 507–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chase, C. C., Shemwell, J. T., & Schwartz, D. L. (2010). Explaining across contrasting cases for deep understanding in science: An example using interactive simulations. In S. Goldman & J. Pellegrino (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th international conference of the learning sciences (pp. 153–160). Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  6. Chi, M. T. H., De Leeuw, N., Chiu, M., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18(3), 439–477.Google Scholar
  7. Day, J., & Bonn, D. (2011). Development of the concise data processing assessment. Physical Review Special Topics Physics Education Research, 7(1), 010114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Day, J., Nakahara, H., & Bonn, D. (2010). Teaching standard deviation by building from student invention. The Physics Teacher, 48, 546–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. de Jong, T. (2006). Scaffolds for scientific discovery learning. In J. Elen, R. E. Clark, & J. Lowyck (Eds.), Handling complexity in learning environments: Theory and research (pp. 107–128). Howard House: Emerald Group Publishing.Google Scholar
  10. de Jong, T., & van Joolingen, W. R. (1998). Scientific discovery learning with computer simulations of conceptual domains. Review of Educational Research, 68, 179–201.Google Scholar
  11. Ge, X., & Land, S. M. (2003). Scaffolding students’ problem-solving processes in an ill-structured task using question prompts and peer interactions. Educational Technology Research and Development, 51(1), 21–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2003). Theory and reality: An introduction to the philosophy of science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  13. Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 379–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kapur, M. (2009). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science, 38(6), 523–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kapur, M. (2010). A further study of productive failure in mathematical problem solving: Unpacking the design components. Instructional Science, 39(4), 561–579. doi:10.1007/s11251-010-9144-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kapur, M. (2012). Productive failure in learning the concept of variance. Instructional Science (this issue).Google Scholar
  17. Kapur, M., & Bielaczyc, K. (2011). Classroom-based experiments in productive failure. In L. Carlson, C. Hölscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2812–2817). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  18. Kapur, M., & Bielaczyc, K. (2012). Designing for productive failure. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(1), 45–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction—effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15(10), 661–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Koedinger, K. R., Aleven, V., Roll, I., & Baker, R. S. J.d. (2009). In vivo experiments on whether supporting metacognition in intelligent tutoring systems yields robust learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 383–412). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  22. Lovett, M. (1998). Cognitive task analysis in service of intelligent tutoring system design: A case study in statistics. In B. P. Goettl, et al. (Eds.), Intelligent Tutoring Systems Lecture Notes in Computer Science (pp. 234–243). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  23. Manlove, S., Lazonder, A. W., & de Jong, T. (2007). Software scaffolds to promote regulation during scientific inquiry learning. Metacognition and Learning, 2, 141–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mathan, S. A., & Koedinger, K. R. (2005). Fostering the intelligent novice: Learning from errors with metacognitive tutoring. Educational Psychologist, 40(4), 257–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McDaniel, M. A., & Schlager, M. S. (1990). Discovery learning and transfer of problem-solving skills. Cognition and Instruction, 7(2), 129–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mulder, Y. G., Lazonder, A. W., & de Jong, T. (2009). Finding out how they find it out: An empirical analysis of inquiry learners’ need for support. International Journal of Science Education, 1–21.Google Scholar
  27. Nathan, M. J. (1998). Knowledge and situational feedback in a learning environment for algebra story problem solving. Interactive Learning Environments, 5(1), 135–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Needham, D. R., & Begg, I. M. (1991). Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogical transfer: Memory-oriented training promotes memory for training. Memory and Cognition, 19(6), 543–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Osman, M. E., & Hannafin, M. J. (1994). Effects of advance questioning and prior knowledge on science learning. The Journal of Educational Research, 88(1), 5–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Roll, I., Aleven, V., & Koedinger, K. R. (2009). Helping students know ‘further’—increasing the flexibility of students’ knowledge using symbolic invention tasks. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1169–1174). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  31. Roll, I., Aleven, V., & Koedinger, K. R. (2011a). Outcomes and mechanisms of transfer. In L. Carlson, C. Hölscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2824–2829). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  32. Roll, I., Aleven, V., McLaren, B. M., & Koedinger, K. R. (2007). Designing for metacognition—applying cognitive tutor principles to the tutoring of help seeking. Metacognition and Learning, 2(2), 125–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Roll, I., Aleven, V., McLaren, B. M., & Koedinger, K. R. (2011b). Improving students’ help-seeking skills using metacognitive feedback in an intelligent tutoring system. Learning and Instruction, 21, 267–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1987). What’s all the fuss about metacognition? Cognitive Science and Mathematics Education (pp. 189–215). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  35. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp. 334–370). New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  36. Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16(4), 475–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 129–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Schwartz, D. L., Martin, T., & Pfaffman, J. (2005). How mathematics propels the development of physical knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(1), 65–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schwartz, D. L., Sears, D., & Chang, J. (2007). Reconsidering prior knowledge. In M. C. Lovett & P. Shah (Eds.), Thinking With Data (pp. 319–344). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  40. Siegler, R. S. (2002). Microgenetic studies of self-explanation. In N. Granott & J. Parziale (Eds.), Microdevelopment—Transition Processes In Development And Learning (pp. 30–58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2009). It pays to compare: An experimental study on computational estimation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(4), 408–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Taylor, J. L., Smith, K. M., van Stolk, A. P., & Spiegelman, G. B. (2010). Using invention to change how students tackle problems. CBE Life Sciences Education, 9(4), 504–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Terwel, J., van Oers, B., van Dijk, I., & van den Eeden, P. (2009). Are representations to be provided or generated in primary mathematics education? Effects on transfer. Educational Research and Evaluation, 15(1), 25–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tobias, S., & Duffy, T. M. (2009). Constructivist Instruction: Success or Failure? New York: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
  45. Westermann, K., & Rummel, N. (2012). Delaying instruction—evidence from a study in a university relearning setting. Instructional Science. doi:10.1007/s11251-012-9207-8.
  46. White, B. Y., & Frederiksen, J. R. (1998). Inquiry, modeling, and metacognition: Making science accessible to all students. Cognition and Instruction, 16(1), 3–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wiedmann, M., Leach, R. C., Rummel, N., & Wiley, J. (2012). Does group composition affect learning by invention? Instructional Science. doi:10.1007/s11251-012-9204-y.

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ido Roll
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Natasha G. Holmes
    • 2
  • James Day
    • 2
  • Doug Bonn
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special EducationUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada
  2. 2.Department of Physics and AstronomyUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada
  3. 3.VancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations