Instructional Science

, Volume 39, Issue 6, pp 805–819

Effects of epistemological sensitization on source choices

Article

Abstract

The uneven distribution of knowledge within modern societies requires a reliance on sources (e.g., reference books, teachers, the Internet) in addition to own experience. Most scientific issues are far too complex to be understood in any depth by laypersons. Successful knowledge acquisition comprises the ability to vary the amount of sources used and to appreciate different sources in different contexts. Epistemological beliefs (i.e., learners’ beliefs on the nature of knowledge and knowing) might affect this task. We report research on secondary school students’ decisions about the amount of sources they would use and their source appreciation (the perceived usefulness of several sources), the impact of epistemological beliefs on such decisions, and their adaptivity to the contextual circumstances of the information search. After exposure to an experimentally induced epistemological sensitization, the students provided information on how they would proceed to confirm given knowledge. Study 1 explored the amount of sources (source quantity and access approaches). Study 2 investigated the perceived usefulness of several sources (source appreciation). Results showed an increase in the amount and appreciation of sources in a high-involving context and after sophisticated sensitization. More sophisticated beliefs led to a larger increase in the amount of sources in a high-involving context. Furthermore, epistemological beliefs predicted the difference in source appreciation between contexts. Conclusions are drawn for the use of sensitization and the teaching of digital literacy in schools.

Keywords

Information sources Source evaluation Epistemological beliefs Learning context Division of cognitive labor 

References

  1. Baram-Tsabari, A., Sethi, R. J., Bry, L., & Yarden, A. (2009). Asking scientists: A decade of questions analyzed by age, gender and country. Science Education, 93(1), 131–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bergstrom, B., Moehlmann, B., & Boyer, P. (2006). Extending the testimony problem: Evaluating the truth, scope, and source of cultural information. Child Development, 77(3), 531–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bordia, P., DiFonzo, N., Haines, R., & Chaseling, E. (2005). Rumors denials as persuasive messages: Effects of personal relevance, source, and message characteristics. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1301–1331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bortz, J., Lienert, G. A., & Boehnke, K. (2008). Verteilungsfreie Methoden in der Biostatistik [Nonparametric methods in biostatistics]. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  5. Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J. F. (2010). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: Towards an integrated model. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  6. Britt, M. A., & Aglinskas, C. (2002). Improving student’s ability to use source information. Cognition and Instruction, 20(40), 485–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bromme, R., Kienhues, D., & Stahl, E. (2008). Knowledge and epistemological beliefs: An intimate but complicate relationship. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 423–444). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  8. Bromme, R., Kienhues, D., & Porsch, T. (2010). Who knows what and who can we believe? Epistemological beliefs are beliefs about knowledge (mostly) to be attained from others. In L. D. Bendixen & F. C. Haerle (Eds.), Personal epistemology in the classroom: Theory, research, and implications for practice (pp. 163–193). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buehl, M. M., & Alexander, P. A. (2006). Examining the dual nature of epistemological beliefs. International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 28–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 460–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Coiro, J., Knobel, M., Lankshear, C., & Leu, D. J. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of research on new literacies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Goldman, S. R., Lawless, K. A., Gomez, K. W., Braasch, J. B., Macleod, S., & Manning, F. (2010). Literacy in the digital world: Comprehending and learning from multiple sources. In M. G. McKeown & L. Kucan (Eds.), Threads of coherence in research on the development of reading ability, or bringing reading researchers to life (pp. 257–284). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Hammer, D., & Elby, A. (2002). On the form of a personal epistemology. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 169–190). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Hofer, B. K. (2004). Epistemological understanding as a metacognitive process: Thinking aloud during online searching. Educational Psychologist, 39, 43–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88–140.Google Scholar
  17. Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Iding, M. K., Crosby, M. E., Auernheimer, B., & Klemm, E. B. (2009). Web site credibility: Why do people believe what they believe? Instructional Science, 37, 43–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kienhues, D., Bromme, R., & Stahl, E. (2008). Changing epistemological beliefs: The unexpected impact of a short-term intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 545–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Mason, L., & Boldrin, A. (2008). Epistemic metacognition in the context of information searching on the web. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge and beliefs: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 377–404). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  21. McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107, 343–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Metzger, M. J., & Flanagin, A. J. (Eds.). (2008). Digital media, youth, and credibility. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion: Theory and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  24. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Involvement and persuasion: Tradition versus integration. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 367–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pieschl, S., Bromme, R., Porsch, T., & Stahl, E. (2008). Epistemological sensitisation causes deeper elaboration during self-regulated learning. International perspectives in the learning sciences: Creating a learning world. Proceedings of the eighth international conference for the learning sciences - ICLS 2008, Vol. 2 (pp. 2,213–2,220). London: Lulu Enterprises.Google Scholar
  26. Porsch, T., Bromme, R., & Pollmeier, J. (2010). Was muss man tun, um sicher die richtige Lösung zu finden? Quellenpräferenzen von Grundschulkindern in verschiedenen Fachkontexten [What needs to be done to find the right answer? Source preferences of elementary school children dealing with tasks from different school subjects]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 42(2), 90–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Rouet, J.-F. (2006). The skills of document use: From text comprehension to web-based learning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  28. Sandoval, W. A. (2005). Understanding students’ practical epistemologies and their influence on learning through inquiry. Science Education, 89(4), 634–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2008). Effects of the metacognitive tool met.a.ware on the web search of laypersons. Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 716–737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Stahl, E., & Bromme, R. (2007). The CAEB: An instrument for measuring connotative aspects of epistemological beliefs. Learning and Instruction, 17(6), 773–785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Steinkuehler, C. (2008). Cognition and literacy in massively multiplayer online games. In D. Leu, J. Coiro, C. Lankshear, & K. Knobel (Eds.), Handbook of research on new literacies (pp. 601–634). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Beyer, B. (2004). Rauchen ist tödlich, Computerspiele machen aggressiv? Allgemeine und theorienspezifische epistemologische Überzeugungen bei Studierenden unterschiedlicher Fachrichtungen [Smoking kills, computer games lead to aggressive behavior? General and theory-specific epistemological beliefs in students of different subjects]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 187–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Whitmire, E. (2004). The relationship between undergraduates’ epistemological beliefs, reflective judgment, and their information-seeking behavior. Information Processing & Management, 40, 97–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wiley, J., Goldman, S., Graesser, A., Sanchez, C., Ash, I., & Hemmerich, J. (2009). Source evaluation, comprehension, and learning in internet science inquiry tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 46, 1060–1106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of MuensterMuensterGermany

Personalised recommendations