Instructional Science

, Volume 38, Issue 3, pp 289–307

The expertise reversal effect and worked examples in tutored problem solving

  • Ron J. C. M. Salden
  • Vincent Aleven
  • Rolf Schwonke
  • Alexander Renkl
Article

Abstract

Prior research has shown that tutored problem solving with intelligent software tutors is an effective instructional method, and that worked examples are an effective complement to this kind of tutored problem solving. The work on the expertise reversal effect suggests that it is desirable to tailor the fading of worked examples to individual students’ growing expertise levels. One lab and one classroom experiment were conducted to investigate whether adaptively fading worked examples in a tutored problem-solving environment can lead to higher learning gains. Both studies compared a standard Cognitive Tutor with two example-enhanced versions, in which the fading of worked examples occurred either in a fixed manner or in a manner adaptive to individual students’ understanding of the examples. Both experiments provide evidence of improved learning results from adaptive fading over fixed fading over problem solving. We discuss how to further optimize the fading procedure matching each individual student’s changing knowledge level.

Keywords

Cognitive tutor Worked examples Adaptive fading Expertise reversal effect 

References

  1. Aleven, V., & Koedinger, K. R. (2002). An effective meta-cognitive strategy: Learning by doing and explaining with a computer-based Cognitive Tutor. Cognitive Science, 26, 147–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aleven, V., McLaren, B. M., Sewall, J., & Koedinger, K. R. (in press). Example-tracing tutors: A new paradigm for intelligent tutoring systems. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson, J. R., Corbett, A. T., Koedinger, K. R., & Pelletier, R. (1995). Cognitive tutors: Lessons learned. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4, 167–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Atkinson, R. K., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Combining fading with prompting fosters learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 774–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baker, R. S. J. d., Corbett, A. T., & Aleven, V. (2008). More accurate student modeling through contextual estimation of slip and guess probabilities in Bayesian knowledge tracing. In B. Woolf, E. Aimeur, R. Nkambou, & S. Lajoie (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th international conference on intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 406–415). Berlin: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
  6. Cen, H., Koedinger, K. R., & Junker, B. (2007). Is over practice necessary?—improving learning efficiency with the Cognitive Tutor through educational data mining. In R. Luckin, K. R. Koedinger, & J. Greer (Eds.), Proceedings of 13th international conference on artificial intelligence in education (AIED2007) (pp. 511–518). Amsterdam: IOS Press.Google Scholar
  7. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  8. Corbett, A. T., & Anderson, J. R. (1995). Knowledge tracing: Modeling the acquisition of procedural knowledge. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 4, 253–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-entailed instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2004). Measuring knowledge to optimize cognitive load factors during instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 558–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2005). Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53, 83–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Koedinger, K. R., & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance dilemma in experiments with Cognitive Tutors. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 239–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Koedinger, K. R., Anderson, J. R., Hadley, W. H., & Mark, M. A. (1997). Intelligent tutoring goes to school in the big city. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 8, 30–43.Google Scholar
  14. Koedinger, K. R., Corbett, A. T., Ritter, S., & Shapiro, L. (2000). Carnegie learning’s cognitive tutor™: Summary research results. (Available from Carnegie Learning, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA): http://www.carnegielearning.com.
  15. McLaren, B. M., Lim, S., Yaron, D., & Koedinger, K. R. (2007). Can a polite intelligent tutoring system lead to improved learning outside of the lab? In R. Luckin, K. R. Koedinger, & J. Greer (Eds), Proceedings of the 13th international conference on artificial intelligence in education (AIED-07), Artificial Intelligence in Education: Building technology rich learning contexts that work (pp. 433–440). IOS Press.Google Scholar
  16. Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2007). An example order for cognitive skill acquisition. In F. E. Ritter, J. Nerb, E. Lehtinen, & T. O’Shea (Eds.), In order to learn: How the sequence of topics influences learning (pp. 95–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Renkl, A., Atkinson, R. K., & Große, C. S. (2004). How fading worked solution steps works—a cognitive load perspective. Instructional Science, 32, 59–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Renkl, A., Atkinson, R. K., Maier, U. H., & Staley, R. (2002). From example study to problem solving: Smooth transitions help learning. Journal of Experimental Education, 70, 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Roy, M., & Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Self-explanation in a multi-media context. In R. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 271–286). Cambridge: Cambridge Press.Google Scholar
  20. Schwonke, R., Wittwer, J., Aleven, V., Salden, R. J. C. M., Krieg, C., & Renkl, A. (2007). Can tutored problem solving benefit from faded worked-out examples? In S. Vosniadou, D. Kayser, & A. Protopapas (Eds.), Proceedings of EuroCogSci 07. The european cognitive science conference 2007 (pp. 59–64). New York, NY: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Van Lehn, K., Lynch, C., Schulze, K., Shapiro, J. A., Shelby, A., Taylor, D., Weinstein, A., & Wintersgill, M. (2005). The Andes physics tutoring project: Five years of evaluations. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 15, 1–47.Google Scholar
  23. Ward, M., & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 1–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ron J. C. M. Salden
    • 1
  • Vincent Aleven
    • 2
  • Rolf Schwonke
    • 3
  • Alexander Renkl
    • 3
  1. 1.Human–Computer Interaction InstituteCarnegie Mellon UniversityPittsburghUSA
  2. 2.Human–Computer Interaction InstituteCarnegie Mellon UniversityPittsburghUSA
  3. 3.Psychological Institute Educational and Developmental PsychologyUniversity of FreiburgFreiburgGermany

Personalised recommendations