Topoi

, Volume 35, Issue 1, pp 57–71 | Cite as

Scalar Implicatures Versus Presuppositions: The View from Acquisition

  • Cory Bill
  • Jacopo Romoli
  • Florian Schwarz
  • Stephen Crain
Article

Abstract

This paper reports an experimental investigation of presuppositions and scalar implicatures in language acquisition. Recent proposals (Chemla 2009; Romoli 2012, Romoli in J Semant 1–47, 2014) posit the same mechanisms for generating both types of inferences, in contrast to the traditional view. We used a Covered Box picture selection task to compare the interpretations assigned by two groups of children (4/5 and 7 year olds) and by adults, in response to sentences with presuppositions and ones with either ‘direct’ or ‘indirect’ scalar implicatures. The main finding was that the behavior of children and adults differed across inference types. This asymmetry is consistent with the traditional perspective, but poses a challenge for the more recent uniform accounts. We discuss how the latter could be amended to account for these findings, and also relate the findings to previous results on presupposition processing. Finally, we discuss an unexpected difference found between direct and indirect scalar implicatures.

Keywords

Presuppositions Scalar implicatures Pragmatics Child language acquisition 

References

  1. Abusch D (2002) Lexical alternatives as a source of pragmatic presupposition. In: Jackson B (ed) Semantics and linguistic theory (SALT) 12. University of California San Diego and San Diego State University, San Diego, pp 1–19Google Scholar
  2. Abusch D (2010) Presupposition triggering from alternatives. J Semant 27:1–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barner D, Brooks N, Bale A (2011) Accessing the unsaid: the role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inferences. Cognition 188:87–96Google Scholar
  4. Beaver D, Geurts B (2012) Presuppositions. In: Maienborn C, von Heusinger K, Portner P (eds) Semantics: an international handbook of natural language meaning, vol 3. Mouton de Gruyter, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  5. Chemla E (2009) Similarity: towards a unified account of scalar implicatures, free choice permission and presupposition projection. Semantics and Pragmatics (in preparation)Google Scholar
  6. Chemla E, Bott L (2013) Processing presuppositions: dynamic semantics vs pragmatic enrichment. Langua Cognit Process 38:241–260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chemla E, Singh R (2014) Remarks on the experimental turn in the study of scalar implicatures, Part 1. Lang Linguist Compass 8:373–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chierchia G (2004) Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena, and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In: Belletti A (ed) Structures and beyond: the cartography of syntactic structures, vol 3. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  9. Crain S, Thornton R (1998) Investigations in universal grammar: a guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  10. Cremers A, Chemla E (2013) Direct and indirect scalar implicatures share the same processing signature. Unpublished manuscript, LSCP-ENS, ParisGoogle Scholar
  11. Foppolo F, Guasti MT, Chierchia G (2012) Scalar implicatures in child language: give children a chance. Lang Learn Dev 8:365–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Grice HP (1975) Logic and conversation. In: Cole P, Morgan JL (eds) Syntax and semantics, vol 3. Seminar Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Heim I (1982) The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Dissertation, University of MassachusettsGoogle Scholar
  14. Heim I (1983) On the projection problem for presuppositions. In: Flickinger DP (ed) Proceedings of WCCFL 2. CSLI Publications, Stanford, pp 114–125Google Scholar
  15. Horn L (1972) On the semantic properties of logical operators in English. Dissertation, University of CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
  16. Huang YT, Snedeker J (2009) Semantic meaning and pragmatic interpretation in 5-year-olds: evidence from real-time spoken language comprehension. Dev Psychol 45:1723–1739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Huang YT, Spelke E, Snedeker J (2013) What exactly do numbers mean? Lang Learn Dev 9:105–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Karttunen L (1974) Presupposition and linguistic context. Theor Linguist 1:181–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Katsos N, Bishop DVM (2011) Pragmatic tolerance: implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature. Cognition 120:67–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Katsos N, Roqueta CA, Estevan RAC, Cummins C (2011) Are children with specific language impairment competent with the pragmatics and logic of quantification? Cognition 119:43–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Magri G (2010) A theory of individual-level predicates based on blind mandatory scalar implicatures. Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyGoogle Scholar
  22. Magri G (2011) Another argument for embedded scalar implicatures based on oddness in DE environments. Semant Pragmat 4:1–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Musolino J, Lidz J (2006) Why children aren’t universally successful with quantification. Linguistics 44:817–852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Noveck I (2001) When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicatures. Cognition 78:165–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Papafragou A, Musolino J (2003) Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognition 86:253–282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Reinhart T (2006) Interface strategies: Optimal and costly derivations. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  27. Roberts C (1996) Information structure in discourse: towards an integrated formal theory of pragmatics. In: Yoon JH, Kathol A (eds) OSU Working Papers in Linguistics 49: Papers in Semantics. The Ohio State University, Columbus, pp 91–136Google Scholar
  28. Romoli J (2012) Soft but strong: neg-raising, soft Triggers, and exhaustification. Dissertation, Harvard UniversityGoogle Scholar
  29. Romoli J (2014) The presuppositions of soft triggers are obligatory scalar implicatures. J Semant 1–47. urlhttp://jos.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2014/01/23/jos.fft017.short
  30. Romoli J, Schwarz F (2015) An experimental comparison between presuppositions and indirect scalar implicatures. In: Schwarz F (ed) Experimental perspectives on presuppositions. Springer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  31. Schlenker P (2008) Be articulate: a pragmatic theory of presupposition projection. Theor Linguist 34:157–212Google Scholar
  32. Schwarz F (2015) Introduction: presuppositions in context - theoretical issues and experimental perspectives. In: Schwarz F (ed) Experimental perspectives on presuppositions. Springer, DordrechtCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schwarz F, Romoli J, Bill C (2014) Processing scalar implicatures: Slowly accepting the truth (literally). Paper presented at the 19th annual meeting of Sinn und Bedeutung, Georg August University, Göttingen, 15–17 September 2014Google Scholar
  34. Simons M (2001) On the conversational basis of some presuppositions. In: Hastings R, Jackson B, Zvolenszky Z (eds) Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) 11, New York University, pp 431–448Google Scholar
  35. Stalnaker R (1974) Pragmatic presuppositions. In: Munitz M, Unger D (eds) Semantics and philosophy. New York University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  36. Sudo Y (2012) On the semantics of phi features on pronouns. Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyGoogle Scholar
  37. von Fintel K (2008) What is accommodation, again? Philoso Perspect 22:137–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Romoli J, Sauerland U (to appear) Presupposition and accommodation. In: Barron A, Steen G, Yueguo G (eds) Handbook of Pragmatics. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  39. Tieu L, Romoli J, Zhou P, Crain S (to appear) Children’s knowledge of free choice inferences. J SemantGoogle Scholar
  40. Zhou P, Romoli J, Crain S (2013) Chidren’s knowledge of alternatives. In: Snider T (ed) Semantics and linguistic theory (SALT) 23. University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, pp 632–651Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cory Bill
    • 1
  • Jacopo Romoli
    • 2
  • Florian Schwarz
    • 3
  • Stephen Crain
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of Cognitive ScienceMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia
  2. 2.School of CommunicationUniversity of UlsterBelfastUK
  3. 3.Department of LinguisticsUniversity of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphiaUSA
  4. 4.Department of LinguisticsMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations