Advertisement

Topoi

, Volume 34, Issue 2, pp 453–459 | Cite as

A Developmental Perspective on Moral Emotions

  • Tina Malti
  • Sebastian P. Dys
Article

Abstract

This article outlines a developmental approach to the study of moral emotions. Specifically, we describe our developmental model on moral emotions, one in which emotions and cognitions about morality get increasingly integrated and coordinated with development, while acknowledging inter-individual variation in developmental trajectories across the lifespan. We begin with a conceptual clarification of the concept of moral emotions. After a brief review of our own developmental approach to the study of moral emotions, we provide a selective summary of the developmental literature on negatively and positively valenced moral emotions, and describe a recent line of developmental research on the anticipation of negatively and positively valenced moral emotions in multifaceted contexts of social exclusion and inclusion. Next, we describe what it means to take a developmental perspective on moral emotions. Finally, we provide some preliminary conclusions and recommendations for future work to the developmental study of moral emotions.

Keywords

Moral emotions Guilt Developmental model Childhood Adolescence 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. Special thanks and appreciation go to the Research Assistants at the Laboratory for Social-emotional Development and Intervention at the University of Toronto, to the families, teachers, and practitioners who dedicated their time to participate in our research, and to our research collaborators.

References

  1. Arsenio WF (2013) Moral emotion attributions and aggression. In: Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development, 2nd edn. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp 235–255Google Scholar
  2. Blair RJR, Fowler KA (2008) Moral emotions and moral reasoning from the perspective of affective cognitive neuroscience: a selective review. Eur J Dev Sci 2:303–322Google Scholar
  3. Blasi A (1980) Bridging moral cognition and moral action: a critical review of the literature. Psychol Bull 88:1–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carlo G (2006) Care-based and altruistically-based morality. In: Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 551–579Google Scholar
  5. Carpendale JIM, Lewis C (2006) How children develop social understanding. Blackwell Publishers, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  6. Davidov M, Zahn-Waxler C, Roth-Hanania R, Knafo A (2013) Concern for others in the first year of life: theory, evidence, and avenues for future research. Child Development Perspectives 7(2):126–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Decety J, Michalska KJ, Kinzler KD (2012) The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. Cereb Cortex 22(1):209–220. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr111 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dys SP, Malti T (2013) Children’s spontaneous and self-reported emotions following moral transgressions. Manuscript in preparationGoogle Scholar
  9. Eisenberg N (2000) Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu Rev Psychol 51(1):665–697. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Morris A (2013) Empathy-related responding in children. In Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development, 2nd edn. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp 184–207Google Scholar
  11. Ekman P, Friesen WV (1969) A tool for the analysis of motion picture film or video tape. Am Psychol 24:240–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Flack JC, de Waal FBM (2000) Any animal whatever? Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7:1–29Google Scholar
  13. Frick PJ, White SF (2008) The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49(4):359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gasser L, Keller M (2009) Are the competent the morally good? Perspective taking and moral motivation of children involved in bullying. Soc Dev 18:798–816. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00516.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gasser L, Malti T, Buholzer A (2013) Swiss children’s moral and psychological judgments about inclusion and cxclusion of children with disabilities. Child Dev. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12124 Google Scholar
  16. Gibbard A (1990) Wise choices, apt feelings: a theory of normative judgment. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger E, Gasser L, Malti T (2010) Moral emotions and moral judgment in children’s narratives: Comparing real-life and hypothetical transgressions. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev 129:11–32. doi: 10.1002/cd.273 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hamlin K, Wynn K, Bloom P (2007) Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450:557–559. doi: 10.1038/nature06288 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hoffman ML (2000) Empathy and moral development. Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Izard CE (1994) Minimal cognitive prerequisites for emotion. In: Ekman P, Davidson RJ (eds) The nature of emotion. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 356–361Google Scholar
  21. Johnston M, Krettenauer T (2011) Moral self and moral emotion expectancies as predictors of anti- and prosocial behavior in adolescence: a case for mediation? Eur J Dev Psychol 8(2):228–243. doi: 10.1080/17405621003619945 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Keller M, Lourenço S, Malti T, Saalbach H (2003) The multifaceted phenomenon of “happy victimizers”: a cross-cultural comparison of moral emotions. Br J Dev Psychol 21:1–18. doi: 10.1348/026151003321164582 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Killen M (2007) Children’s social and moral reasoning about exclusion. Curr Dir in Psychol Sci 16:32–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00470.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Killen M, Lee-Kim J, McGlothlin H, Stangor C (2002) How children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 67(4, Serial No. 271)Google Scholar
  25. Killen M, Mulvey KL, Richardson C, Jampol N, Woodward A (2011) The accidental transgressor: morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition 119(2):197–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Knafo A, Plomin R (2006) Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Dev Psychol 42(5):771–786. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.771 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Knafo A, Zahn-Waxler C, Van Hulle C, Robinson JL, Rhee SH (2008) The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion 8(6):737–752. doi: 10.1037/a0014179 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kochanska G, Barry RA, Jimenez NB, Hollatz AL, Woodard J (2009) Guilt and effortful control: two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. J Pers Soc Psychol 97(2):322–333. doi: 10.1037/a0015471 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kohlberg L (1984) The psychology of moral development: Volume 2. Essays on moral development. Harper and Row, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  30. Krettenauer T, Eichler D (2006) Adolescents’ self-attributed emotions following a moral transgression: Relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment, and age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 24:489–506. doi: 10.1348/026151005X50825 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Krettenauer T, Malti T, Sokol BW (2008) Development of moral emotions and the happy-victimizer phenomenon: A critical review of theory and application. Eur J Dev Sci 2:221–235. doi: 10.3233/DEV-2008-2303 Google Scholar
  32. Lagattuta KH, Thompson RA (2007) The development of self-conscious emotions: cognitive processes and social influences. In: Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP (eds) The self-conscious emotions: theory and research. Guilford, New York, pp 91–113Google Scholar
  33. Leslie AM, Knobe J, Cohen A (2006) Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect: theory of mind and moral judgment. Psychol Sci 17(5):421–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01722.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Malti T (2013, in preparation) Toward an integrated clinical-developmental model of moral emotions in early and middle childhoodGoogle Scholar
  35. Malti T, Keller M (2010) Development of moral emotions in cultural context. In: Arsenio W, Lemerise E (eds) Emotions, aggression, and morality in children: bridging development and psychpathology. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 177–198. doi: 10.1037/12129-009
  36. Malti T, Krettenauer T (2013) The relation of moral emotion attributions to prosocial and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 84(2):397–412. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01851.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Malti T, Latzko B (2012) Moral emotions. In: Ramachandran V (ed) Encyclopedia of human behavior. Elsevier, Maryland Heights,2nd ed, pp 644–649. doi:  10.1016/B978-0-12-375000-6.00099-9
  38. Malti T, Ongley SF (2013) The development of moral emotions and moral reasoning. In: Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development, 2nd edn. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp 163–183Google Scholar
  39. Malti T, Gummerum M, Keller M, Buchmann M (2009) Children’s moral motivation, sympathy, and prosocial behavior. Child Dev 80:442–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01271.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Malti T, Gummerum M, Keller M, Chaparro MP, Buchmann M (2012a) Early sympathy and social acceptance predict the development of sharing in children. PLoS ONE 7(12):e52017. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Malti T, Keller M, Buchmann M (2012b) Do moral choices make us feel good? The development of adolescents’ emotions following moral decision making. J Res Adolesc 23(2):389–397. doi: 10.1111/jora.12005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Malti T, Killen M, Gasser L (2012c) Social judgments and emotion attributions about exclusion in Switzerland. Child Dev 83:697–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01705.x Google Scholar
  43. Malti T, Eisenberg N, Kim H, Buchmann M (2013) Developmental trajectories of sympathy, moral emotion attributions, and moral reasoning: the role of parental support. Soc Dev. doi: 10.1111/sode.12031 Google Scholar
  44. Nunner-Winkler G, Sodian B (1988) Children’s understanding of moral emotions. Child Dev 59:1323–1338. doi: 10.2307/1130495 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nussbaum M (2001) Upheavals of thought: the intelligence of the emotions. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Oakley B, Knafo A, Madhavan G, Wilson DS (eds) (2011) Pathological altruism. Oxford University Press, USAGoogle Scholar
  47. Oser F (2006) The “unhappy moralist” effect: emotional conflicts between being good and being successful. In: Verschaffel L, Dochy F, Boekaerts M, Vosniadou S (eds) Instructional psychology: past, present and future trends. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 149–166Google Scholar
  48. Piaget J (1932/1999) The moral development of the child. Kegan Paul, LondonGoogle Scholar
  49. Roth-Hanania R, Davidov M, Zahn-Waxler C (2011) Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: early signs of concern for others. Infant Behav Dev 34(3):447–458. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sinnot-Armstrong WP (ed) (2008a) Moral psychology, volume 2: The cognitive science of morality. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  51. Sinnot-Armstrong WP (ed) (2008b) Moral psychology, volume 3: The neuroscience of morality. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  52. Smetana JG (2006) Social-cognitive domain theory: consistencies and variations in children’s moral and social judgments. In: Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 119–153Google Scholar
  53. Sokol BW (2004) Children’s conceptions of agency and morality: making sense of the happy victimizer phenomenon. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, VancouverGoogle Scholar
  54. Sokol BW, Chandler MJ (2003) Taking agency seriously in the theories-of-mind enterprise: exploring children’s understanding of interpretation and intention. Br J Educ Psychol Monog Ser II:125–136Google Scholar
  55. Strohmeier D, Malti T, Killen M (2013) The role of bystander behavior on judgments and emotion attributions about social exclusion in Austrian schools. Manuscript in preparationGoogle Scholar
  56. Tangney JP, Stuewig J, Mashek DJ (2007) Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 58:345–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP (2007) The self-conscious emotions: theory and research. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  58. Turiel E (1983) The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  59. Wainryb C, Recchia HE (2012) Emotion and the moral lives of adolescents: vagaries and complexities in the emotional experience of doing harm. New Dir Youth Dev 136:13–26. doi: 10.1002/yd.20035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wainryb C, Brehl BA, Matwin S (2005) Being hurt and hurting others: children’s narrative accounts and moral judgments of their own interpersonal conflicts. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 70:1–114Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of TorontoMissisaugaCanada

Personalised recommendations