Topoi

, Volume 33, Issue 1, pp 185–199 | Cite as

Social Constraints on the Direct Perception of Emotions and Intentions

Article

Abstract

In this paper, we first review recent arguments about the direct perception of the intentions and emotions of others, emphasizing the role of embodied interaction. We then consider a possible objection to the direct perception hypothesis from social psychology, related to phenomena like ‘dehumanization’ and ‘implicit racial bias’, which manifest themselves on a basic bodily level. On the background of such data, one might object that social perception cannot be direct since it depends on and can in fact be interrupted by a set of cultural beliefs. We argue, however, that far from threatening the idea of direct perception, these findings clearly contradict the idea of hardwired theory of mind modules. More generally, we suggest that in order to further the understanding of social cognition we must take seriously insights about in-group and out-group distinctions and related phenomena, all of which are currently neglected in the mainstream social cognition literature.

Keywords

Direct perception Theory theory Simulation theory Interaction theory Embodiment Dehumanization 

References

  1. Ansuini C, Santello M, Massaccesi S, Castiello U (2006) Effects of end-goal on hand shaping. J Neurophysiol 95:2456–2465Google Scholar
  2. Ansuini C, Giosa L, Turella L, Altoè GM, Castiello U (2008) An object for an action, the same object for other actions: effects on hand shaping. Exp Brain Res 185:111–119Google Scholar
  3. Atkinson AP, Tunstall ML, Dittrich WH (2007) Evidence for distinct contributions of form and motion information to the recognition of emotions from body gestures. Cognition 104:59–72Google Scholar
  4. Bain P, Park J, Kwok C, Haslam N (2009) Attributing Human Uniqueness and Human Nature to Cultural Groups: distinct forms of subtle dehumanization. Group Process Intergroup Relat 12:789Google Scholar
  5. Barden J, Maddux WW, Petty RE, Brewer MB (2004) Contextual moderation of racial bias: the impact of social roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol 87:5Google Scholar
  6. Bargh JA (1999) The cognitive monster: the case against the controllability of automatic stereotype effects. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y (eds) Dual-process theories in social psychology. Guilford Press, New York, pp 361–382Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen S, Swettenham J (1996) The relationship between SAM and ToMM: two hypotheses. In: Carruthers P, Smith PK (eds) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 158–168Google Scholar
  8. Bastian B, Haslam N (2010) Excluded from humanity: the dehumanizing effects of social ostracism. J Exp Soc Psychol 46:107–113Google Scholar
  9. Becchio C, Manera V, Sartori L, Cavallo A, Castiello U (2012) Grasping intentions: from thought experiments to empirical evidence. Front Hum Neurosci 6:117Google Scholar
  10. Bennett MR, Hacker PMS (2008) History of cognitive neuroscience. Wiley-Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  11. Blair IV (2002) The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 6:242–261Google Scholar
  12. Brandom R (2008) Between saying and doing: towards an analytic pragmatism. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  13. Bohl V, Gangopadhyay N (2013) Theory of mind and the unobservability of other minds. Phil Explor. doi:10.1080/13869795.2013.821515
  14. Bratman M (1987) Intention, plans, and practical reason. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  15. Brewer MB, Silver MD (2000) Group distinctiveness, social identification, and collective mobilization. In: Stryker S, Owens TJ, White RW (eds) Self, identity, and social movements. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, MN, pp 153–171Google Scholar
  16. Brewer MB, Manzi JM, Shaw JS (1993) In-group identification as a function of depersonalization, distinctiveness, and status. Psychol Sci 4(2):88–92Google Scholar
  17. Carruthers P (2013) Mindreading in infancy. Mind and Language 28(2):141–172Google Scholar
  18. Cassam K (2007) The possibility of knowledge. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  19. Coulter J, Sharrock W (2009) Theory of mind. In: Leudar I, Costall A (eds) Against theory of mind. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke, pp 56–88Google Scholar
  20. De Jaegher H, Di Paolo E, Gallagher S (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cogn Sci 14:441–447Google Scholar
  21. Debruille JB, Brodeur MB et al (2012) N300 and social affordances: a study with a real person and a dummy as stimuli. PLoS One 7(10):e47922Google Scholar
  22. Dretske F (1973) Perception and other minds. Nous 7:34–44Google Scholar
  23. Duddington NA (1918) Our knowledge of other minds. Proc Aristot Soc 19:147–178Google Scholar
  24. Elfenbein HA, Ambady N (2002a) Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition? Psychol Bull 128:243–249Google Scholar
  25. Elfenbein HA, Ambady N (2002b) On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 128:203–235Google Scholar
  26. Elfenbein HA, Ambady N (2003) When familiarity breeds accuracy: cultural exposure and facial emotion recognition. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:276–290Google Scholar
  27. Elfenbein HA, Beaupré M, Le′vesque M, Hess U (2007) Toward a dialect theory: cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion 7:131–146Google Scholar
  28. Fiske AP (1991) Structures of social life: the four elementary forms of human relations. Free Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  29. Fiske AP (2004) Four modes of constituting relationships: Consubstantial assimilation; space, magnitude, time, and force; concrete procedures; abstract symbolism. In: Haslam N (ed) Relational models theory: a contemporary overview. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Mahwah, NJ, pp 61–146Google Scholar
  30. Fodor Jerry A (1983) Modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  31. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G (2005) Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308:662–667Google Scholar
  32. Frith CD, Frith U (2007) Social cognition in humans. Curr Biol 17:R724–R732Google Scholar
  33. Gallagher S (2001) The practice of mind: theory, simulation, or interaction? J Conscious Stud 8(5–7):83–107Google Scholar
  34. Gallagher S (2004) Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philos Psychiatry Psychol 11(3):199–217Google Scholar
  35. Gallagher S (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  36. Gallagher S (2008a) Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Conscious Cogn 17:535–543Google Scholar
  37. Gallagher S (2008b) Intersubjectivity in perception. Cont Philos Rev 41(2):163–178Google Scholar
  38. Gallagher S (2012) In defense of phenomenological approaches to social cognition: interacting with the critics. Rev Philos Psychol 3(2):187–212Google Scholar
  39. Gallagher S (2013a) The socially extended mind. Cogn Syst Res 25–26:4–12Google Scholar
  40. Gallagher S (2013b) Enactive hands. In: Radman Z (ed) The hand: an organ of the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 209–225Google Scholar
  41. Gallagher S (in press) To follow a rule: lessons from baby logic. In: Weinstock D, Levy J (eds) Essays in honor of Charles Taylor. McGill-Queen’s University Press, MontrealGoogle Scholar
  42. Gallagher S, Hutto D (2008) Primary interaction and narrative practice. In: Zlatev J, Racine T, Sinha C, Itkonen E (eds) The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 17–38Google Scholar
  43. Gallagher S, Zahavi D (2008) The phenomenological mind. Routlege, LondonGoogle Scholar
  44. Gallese V (2006) Intentional attunement: a neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Res. Cog. Brain Res. 1079:15–24Google Scholar
  45. Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW (2000) Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nat Neurosci 3:191–197Google Scholar
  46. Gegenfurtner K, Olkkonen M, Walter S (2006) Memory modulates color experience. Nat Neurosci 9(11):1367–1368Google Scholar
  47. Goffman E (1986) Stigma: notes on the management of spoiled identity. Simon & Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  48. Goh JO, Park DC (2009) Culture sculpts the perceptual brain. Prog Brain Res 178:95–111Google Scholar
  49. Goldman A (2006) Simulating minds: the philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  50. Goldman AI, Sripada CS (2005) Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition 94(2005):193–213Google Scholar
  51. Good JMM (1985) The perception of social actions from point light displays: an exploratory study. Presented at the third international conference on event perception and action, Trieste, ItalyGoogle Scholar
  52. Gordon R (2008) Beyond mindreading. Phil Explor 11:219–222Google Scholar
  53. Green M (2007) Self-expression. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  54. Green M (2010) Perceiving emotions. Aristot Soc Suppl Vol 84:45–61Google Scholar
  55. Gutsell JN, Inzlicht M (2010) Empathy constrained: prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. J Exp Soc Psychol 46:841–845Google Scholar
  56. Harris LT, Fiske ST (2006) Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: neuroimaging responses to extreme outgroups. Psychol Sci 17:847–853Google Scholar
  57. Harris PL, Johnson CN, Hutton D, Andrews G, Cooke T (1989) Young children’s theory of mind and emotion. Cogn Emot 3:379–400Google Scholar
  58. Haslam N (2006) Dehumanization: an integrative review. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 10:252–264Google Scholar
  59. Haslam N, Bain P (2007) Humanizing the self: moderators of the attribution of lesser humanness to others. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 33:57–68Google Scholar
  60. Haslam N, Bain P, Douge L, Lee M, Bastian B (2005) More human than you: attributing humanness to self and others. J Pers Soc Psychol 89:937–950Google Scholar
  61. Haslam N, Kashima Y, Loughnan S, Shi J, Suitner C (2008a) Subhuman, inhuman, and superhuman: contrasting humans with nonhumans in three cultures. Soc Cogn 26:248–258Google Scholar
  62. Haslam N, Loughnan S, Kashima Y, Bain P (2008b) Attributing and denying humanness to others. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 19:55–85Google Scholar
  63. Heal J (1995) How to think about thinking. In: Davies M, Stone T (eds) Mental simulation. Oxford, Blackwell, pp 33–52Google Scholar
  64. Helmholtz H (1867) Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. Leopold Voss, LeipzigGoogle Scholar
  65. Hobson P (2002) The cradle of thought. Macmillan, LondonGoogle Scholar
  66. Hutto D, Myin E (2013) Radicalizing enactivism: basic minds without content. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  67. Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 3(3):529–535Google Scholar
  68. Izard CE (1972) Patterns of emotions: a new analysis of anxiety and depression. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  69. Izard CE, Ackerman BP, Schoff KM, Fine SE (2000) Self-organization of discrete emotions, emotion patterns, and emotion cognition relations. In: Lewis MD, Granic I (eds) Emotion, development, and self-organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 15–36Google Scholar
  70. Jacob P (2011) The direct perception model of empathy: a critique. Rev Philos Psychol 2:519–540Google Scholar
  71. Johnson SC (2000) The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy. Trends Cogn Sci 4:22–28Google Scholar
  72. Kaplan JT, Iacoboni M (2006) Getting a grip on other minds: mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy. Soc Neurosci 1(3–4):175–183Google Scholar
  73. Kitayama S, Park J (2010) Cultural neuroscience of the self: understanding the social grounding of the brain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5(2–3):111–129Google Scholar
  74. Krueger J (2012) Seeing mind in action. Phenomenol Cognitive Sci 11(2):149–173Google Scholar
  75. Krueger J, Overgaard S (2012) Seeing subjectivity: defending a perceptual account of other minds. ProtoSociol: Conscious Subj 47:239–262Google Scholar
  76. Latham AJ, Patston LL, Tippett LJ (2013) The virtual brain: 30 years of video-game play and cognitive abilities. Front Psychol 4(629). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00629
  77. Lavelle JS (2012) Theory-theory and the direct perception of mental states. Rev Philos Psychol 3(2):213–230Google Scholar
  78. Legerstee M (2005) Infants’ Sense of People: Precursors to a Theory of Mind. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  79. Leudar I, Costall A (2004) On the persistence of the ‘problem of other minds’ in psychology: chomsky, grice and ‘theory of mind’. Theory Psychol 14:601–621Google Scholar
  80. Leyens Jacques-Philippe, Yzerbyt Vincent, Schadron Georges (1994) Stereotypes and social cognition. Sage Publications, LondonGoogle Scholar
  81. Likowski KU, Mühlberger A, Seibt B, Pauli P, Weyers P (2008) Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:1065–1072Google Scholar
  82. Lindblom J (2007) Minding the body: interacting socially through embodied action. Linköping: linköping studies in science and technology, Dissertation No. 1112, Department of Computer and Information Science, Linköpings Universitet, SwedenGoogle Scholar
  83. Malcolm N (1953) Direct perception. The Philosophical Quarterly 3(13):301–316Google Scholar
  84. Manera V, Becchio C, Cavallo A, Sartori L, Castiello U (2011a) Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements. Exp Brain Res 211:547–556Google Scholar
  85. Manera V, Becchio C, Schouten B, Bara BG, Verfaillie K (2011b) Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS One 6:e145–e194Google Scholar
  86. Manera V, Del Giudice M, Bara BG, Verfaillie K, Becchio C (2011c) The second-agent effect: communicative gestures increase the likelihood of perceiving a second agent. PLoS One 6:e226–e250Google Scholar
  87. Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M, Athenes S, Dugas C (1987) Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Can J Psychol 41:365–378Google Scholar
  88. Matsumoto D (2002) Methodological requirements to test a possible in-group advantage in judging emotions across cultures. Psychol Bull 128:236–242Google Scholar
  89. McDowell J (2011) Some remarks on intention in action. Amherst Lect Philos 6:1–18Google Scholar
  90. McNeill W (2012) Embodiment and the perceptual hypothesis. Philos Quart 62(248):569–591Google Scholar
  91. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1989) Imitation in newborn infants: exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Dev Psychol 25(6):954–962Google Scholar
  92. Meltzoff A, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:75–78Google Scholar
  93. Meltzoff A, Moore MK (1994) Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behav Dev 17:83–99Google Scholar
  94. Mendoça D (2012) Pattern of sentiment: following a Deweyan suggestion. Trans Charles S Peirce Soc 48(2):209–227Google Scholar
  95. Merleau-Ponty M (1964) Sense and non-sense. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  96. Merleau-Ponty M (2002) The phenomenology of perception. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  97. Molnar-Szakacs I, Wu AD, Robles FJ, Iacoboni M (2007) Do you see what I mean? Corticospinal excitability during observation of culture-specific gestures. PLoS One 2:e626Google Scholar
  98. Newen A, Welpinghus A, Jukel G (under review) Emotion recognition as pattern recognition: the relevance of perceptionGoogle Scholar
  99. Nguyen L, Frye D (1999) Children’s theory of mind: understanding of desire, belief and emotion with social referents. Soc Dev 8(1):70–92Google Scholar
  100. Onishi K, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-olds understand false beliefs? Science 308:255–258Google Scholar
  101. Orlandi N (2011) Embedded seeing: Vision in the natural world. Noûs. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0068.2011.00845.x Google Scholar
  102. Pacherie E (2005) Perceiving intentions. In: Sàágua J (ed) A Explicação da Interpretação Humana. Edições Colibri, Lisbon, pp 401–414Google Scholar
  103. Pacherie E (2006) Towards a dynamic theory of intentions. In: Pockett S, Banks WP, Gallagher S (eds) Does Consciousness Cause Behavior? An Investigation of the Nature of Volition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 145–167Google Scholar
  104. Pacherie E (2008) The phenomenology of action: a conceptual framework. Cognition 107:179–217Google Scholar
  105. Park J, Kitayama S (2012) Interdependent selves show face-induced facilitation of error processing: cultural neuroscience of self-threat. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. doi:10.1093/scan/nss125 Google Scholar
  106. Pylyshyn Z (1999) Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behav Brain Sci 22(3):341–365Google Scholar
  107. Reddy V (1991) Playing with others’ expectations: teasing and mucking about in the first year. In: Whiten A (ed) Natural Theories of Mind. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 143–158Google Scholar
  108. Reddy V (2008) How Infants Know Minds. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  109. Rorty R (1989) Contingency, irony, and solidarity. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  110. Runeson S, Frykholm G (1983) Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. J Exp Psychol 112:585–615 Google Scholar
  111. Sartori L, Becchio C, Castiello U (2011a) Cues to intention: the role of movement information. Cognition 119:242–252Google Scholar
  112. Sartori L, Straulino E, Castiello U (2011b) How objects are grasped: the interplay between affordances and end-goals. PLoS One 6(9):e25203. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025203 Google Scholar
  113. Sass L, Pienkos E (2013) Faces of intersubjectivity: interpersonal experience in melancholia, mania, and schizophrenia. J Conscious Stud 20(7–8):103–130Google Scholar
  114. Scheler M (1954) The nature of sympathy. London: Routledge. Kegan PaulGoogle Scholar
  115. Scholl BJ, Leslie AM (1999) Modularity, development, and ‘theory of mind’. Mind Lang 14:131–153Google Scholar
  116. Searle J (1983) Intentionality: an essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  117. Segal G (1996) The modularity of theory of mind. In: Carruthers P, Smith P (eds) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 141–157Google Scholar
  118. Shuler MG, Bear MF (2006) Neurons in the primary visual cortex respond differently to a flash of light after it has been paired with a reward, unexpectedly showing that cognitive information is coded at this level in the cortex. Science 311:1606–1609Google Scholar
  119. Siegel S (2011) Cognitive penetrability and perceptual justification. Nous 46(2):201–222Google Scholar
  120. Smith J (2010) Seeing other people. Philoso Phenomenol Res 81(3):731–748Google Scholar
  121. Spaulding S (2010) Embodied cognition and mindreading. Mind and Lang 25:119–140Google Scholar
  122. Strawson G (1994) Mental reality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  123. Stueber KR (2006) Rediscovering empathy: agency, folk psychology, and the human sciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  124. Tajfel H (1981) Human groups and social categories. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  125. Tajfel H, Billig MG, Bundy RP, Flament C (1971) Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur J Soc Psychol 1(2):149–178Google Scholar
  126. Trevarthen CB (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  127. Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ (2001) Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Annual Research Review. The. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42(1):3–48Google Scholar
  128. Wellman HM (1990) The child’s theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  129. Wellman H, Banerjee M (1991) Mind and emotion: children’s understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires. Br J Dev Psychol 9:191–214Google Scholar
  130. Wittgenstein L (1967) Zettel. In: Anscombe GEM, von Wright GH (eds) (G. E. M. Anscombe, Trans.). University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  131. Wittgenstein L (1980) Remarks on the philosophy of psychology. In: von Wright GH, Nyman H (eds) (Vol. II) (C. G. Luckhardt & M. A. E. Aue, Trans). Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  132. Xu Y (2005) Revisiting the role of the fusiform face area in visual expertise. Cereb Cortex 15(8):1234–1242Google Scholar
  133. Xu Y, Zuo X, Wang X, Han S (2009) Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. J Neurosci 29(26):8525–8529Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of MemphisMemphisUSA
  2. 2.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of MemphisMemphisUSA

Personalised recommendations