Topoi

, Volume 33, Issue 2, pp 487–497 | Cite as

Cognitive Science of Religion and the Study of Theological Concepts

Article

Abstract

The cultural transmission of theological concepts remains an underexplored topic in the cognitive science of religion (CSR). In this paper, I examine whether approaches from CSR, especially the study of content biases in the transmission of beliefs, can help explain the cultural success of some theological concepts. This approach reveals that there is more continuity between theological beliefs and ordinary religious beliefs than CSR authors have hitherto recognized: the cultural transmission of theological concepts is influenced by content biases that also underlie the reception of ordinary religious concepts.

Keywords

Cognitive science of religion Evolutionary approaches to culture Content biases Theology 

References

  1. al-Ghazālī. [eleventh century (1963)]. Incoherence of the philosophers. (S.A. Kamali, Trans.) Pakistan Philosophical Congress,LahoreGoogle Scholar
  2. Armstrong M (2011) Extraordinary eschatology: Insights from ordinary theology. In: Astley J, Francis LJ (eds) Exploring ordinary theology. Everyday Christian believing and the church. Ashgate, Farnham, pp 97–105Google Scholar
  3. Astuti R, Harris PL (2008) Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cognit Sci 32:713–740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Augustine [fifth century (1886)]. Psalm 89. In: Schaff P (ed) Exposition on the book of psalms. T & T Clark, Edinburgh and Eerdmans, Grand Rapids, MI, pp 429–441Google Scholar
  5. Banerjee K, Bloom P (2012) Would Tarzan believe in God? Conditions for the emergence of religious belief. Trends Cognit Sci 17:7–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barrett JL (2011) Cognitive science, religion, and theology. From human minds to divine minds. Templeton Press, West ConshohockenGoogle Scholar
  7. Barrett JL, Keil FC (1996) Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cogn Psychol 31:219–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barrett JL, Nyhof MA (2001) Spreading non-natural concepts: the role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. J Cognit Cult 1:69–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Barrett HC, Broesch T, Scott RM, He Z, Baillargeon R, Wu D, Bolz D, Henrich J, Setoh P, Wang J, Laurence S (2013) Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies. Proc Royal Soci B Biol Sci 280:1471–2954Google Scholar
  10. Bering JM, McLeod K, Shackelford T (2005) Reasoning about dead agents reveals possible adaptive trends. Human Nature 16:360–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bloom P (2004) Descartes’ baby. How child development explains what makes us human. Arrow Books, LondonGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyer P (2002) Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Vintage, LondonGoogle Scholar
  13. Claidière N, Sperber D (2007) The role of attraction in cultural evolution. J Cognit Cult 7:89–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. De Cruz H, De Smedt J (2007) The role of intuitive ontologies in scientific understanding—The case of human evolution. Biol Philos 22:351–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. De Cruz H, De Smedt J (2010) Paley’s iPod: the cognitive basis of the design argument within natural theology. Zygon J Religion Sci 45:665–684Google Scholar
  16. Edwards M (2009) Catholicity and heresy in the early church. Ashgate, FarnhamGoogle Scholar
  17. Ehrman BD (2003) Lost Christianities. The battles for scripture and the faiths we never knew. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Faust J (2008) Can religious arguments persuade? Int J Philos Relig 63:71–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gervais WM, Henrich J (2010) The Zeus problem: why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. J Cognit Cult 10:383–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gervais WM, Willard AK, Norenzayan A, Henrich J (2011) The cultural transmission of faith. Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion 41:389–410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Griffel F (2009) Al-Ghazālī’s philosophical theology. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Henrich J, Boyd R (2002) On modeling cognition and culture. Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. J Cognit Cult 2:87–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Heywood B, Bering J (in press). Meant to be: how religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Religion, Brain and BehaviorGoogle Scholar
  24. Hodge KM (2011) On imagining the afterlife. J Cognit Cult 11:367–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hume D (1757) The natural history of religion. In: Four dissertations. A. Millar, London, pp 1–117Google Scholar
  26. Hume D (1779) Dialogues concerning natural religion. Hafner, LondonGoogle Scholar
  27. Irenaeus [second century (1884)] Against heresies (trans: Roberts A, Rambaut WH). T & T Clark, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  28. Jenkins P (2010) Jesus wars. How four patriarchs, three queens, and two emperors decided what Christians would believe for the next 1,500 years. SPCK, LondonGoogle Scholar
  29. Kelemen D (2004) Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychol Sci 15:295–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Legare CH, Gelman SA (2008) Bewitchment, biology, or both: the coexistence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development. Cognit Sci 32:607–642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. McCauley RN (2011) Why religion is natural and science is not. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  32. Mercier H (2012) The social functions of explicit coherence evaluation. Mind Soc 11:81–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mercier H, Sperber D (2011) Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav Brain Sci 34:57–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Murphy N (2011) Immortality versus resurrection in the Christian tradition. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1234:76–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Nähri J (2008) Beautiful reflections: the cognitive and evolutionary foundations of paradise representations. Method Theory Study Religion 20:339–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nichols S (2002) On the genealogy of norms: a case for the role of emotion in cultural evolution. Philos Sci 69:234–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nichols R, Draper P (2013) Diagnosing cognitive biases in philosophy of religion. The MonistGoogle Scholar
  38. Nickerson RS (1998) Confirmation bias: a ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Rev Gen Psychol 2:175–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pospisil LJ (1978) The Kapauku Papuans of West New Guinea. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  40. Rottman J, Kelemen D (2012) Is there such a thing as a Christian child? Evidence of religious beliefs in early childhood. In: McNamara P, Wildman W (eds) Science and the world’s religions: origins and destinies. Praeger, Santa Barbara, pp 205–238Google Scholar
  41. Samarapungavan A, Wiers RW (1997) Children’s thoughts on the origin of species: a study of explanatory coherence. Cognit Sci 21:147–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schwitzgebel E (2013) The crazyist metaphysics of mind. http://www.faculty.ucr.edu/~eschwitz/SchwitzPapers/CrazyMind-130305.pdf
  43. Slone DJ (2004) Theological incorrectness. Why religious people believe what they shouldn’t. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  44. Sperber D (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind Lang 12:67–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. van Inwagen P (1978) The possibility of resurrection. International Journal for Philosophy of Religion 9, 114–121 (reprinted and revised in 1998, The possibility of resurrection and other essays in Christian apologetics. In: van Inwagen P (ed) Westview Press, Boulder, pp 45–51Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Research Foundation Flanders, Institute of PhilosophyUniversity of LeuvenLeuvenBelgium
  2. 2.Faculty of PhilosophyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations