Advertisement

Preferences over procedures and outcomes in judgment aggregation: an experimental study

  • Takuya Sekiguchi
Article
  • 54 Downloads

Abstract

The aggregation of individual judgments on logically connected issues often leads to collective inconsistency. This study examines two collective decision-making procedures designed to avoid such inconsistency—one premise-based and the other conclusion-based. While the relative desirability of the two procedures has been studied extensively from a theoretical perspective, the preference of individuals regarding the two procedures has been less studied empirically. In the present study, a scenario-based questionnaire survey of participant preferences for the two procedures was conducted, taking into consideration prevailing social norms in the society to which the participants belong and the heterogeneity of the participants’ past experiences. Results show that a minority opinion not matching a prevailing social norm is more likely to be supported when the conclusion-based procedure is used. This can be explained by a basic property of the conclusion-based procedure: The procedure does not require voters to reveal their reasons for reaching a particular conclusion. This property proves appealing for participants who have a minority opinion. Such a finding is highly relevant to future studies on strategic behaviors in choosing a collective decision-making procedure.

Keywords

Doctrinal paradox Framing effect Judgment aggregation Mixed-motivation problem Procedural preference Social norm 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Grant-in-Aid for Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Fellows Grant number 13J05358.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethics statement

This experiment was conducted when the author belonged to Japan Society of the Promotion of Science, and Department of Evolutionary Studies of Biosystems, School of Advanced Sciences, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies). This experiment was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of The Graduate University for Advanced Studies with the receipt number 2015008.

References

  1. Blei, D. M., Ng, A. Y., & Jordan, M. I. (2003). Latent Dirichlet allocation. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 3, 993–1022.Google Scholar
  2. Bonnefon, J. F. (2007). How do individuals solve the doctrinal paradox in collective decisions? An empirical investigation. Psychological Science, 18, 753–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bonnefon, J. F. (2010). Behavioral evidence for framing effects in the resolution of the doctrinal paradox. Social Choice and Welfare, 34, 631–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Converting among effect sizes. In M. Borenstein, L. V. Hedges, J. P. T. Higgins, & H. R. Rothstein (Eds.), Introduction to meta-analysis (pp. 45–49). Chichester, UK: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bovens, L. (2006). The doctrinal paradox and the mixed-motivation problem. Analysis, 66, 35–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bovens, L., & Rabinowicz, W. (2006). Democratic answers to complex questions—an epistemic perspective. Synthese, 150, 131–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chow, S. L. (1988). Significance test or effect size? Psychological Bulletin, 103, 105–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Dietrich, F., & List, C. (2007a). Arrow’s theorem in judgment aggregation. Social Choice and Welfare, 29, 19–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dietrich, F., & List, C. (2007b). Strategy-proof judgment aggregation. Economics and Philosophy, 23, 269–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fawcett, T. (2006). An introduction to ROC analysis. Pattern Recognition Letters, 27, 861–874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ganzach, Y., & Schul, Y. (1995). The influence of quantity of information and goal framing on decision. Acta Psychologica, 89, 23–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Griffiths, T. L., & Steyvers, M. (2004). Finding scientific topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America, 101, 5228–5235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Grofman, B. (1985). The accuracy of group majorities for disjunctive and conjunctive decision tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 119–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Grossi, D., & Pigozzi, G. (2014). Judgment aggregation: A primer. Synthesis Lectures on Artificial Intelligence and Machine Learning, 8, 1–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grün, B., & Hornik, K. (2011). topicmodels: An R package for fitting topic models. Journal of Statistical Software, 40, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ishida, M. (2017). RMeCab: Interface to MeCab. R Package version 0.99997.Google Scholar
  18. Kameda, T. (1991). Procedural influence in small-group decision making: Deliberation style and assigned decision rule. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. List, C. (2005). The probability of inconsistencies in complex collective decisions. Social Choice and Welfare, 24, 3–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. List, C. (2006). The discursive dilemma and public reason. Ethics, 116, 362–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. List, C., & Pettit, P. (2002). Aggregating sets of judgments: An impossibility result. Economics and Philosophy, 18, 89–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. List, C., & Puppe, C. (2009). Judgment aggregation: A survey. In P. Anand, C. Puppe, & P. Pattanaik (Eds.), Oxford handbook of rational and social choice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Spiekermann, K. (2013). Judgment aggregation and distributed thinking. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallee-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain (pp. 31–51). London: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2007). Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 225–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 259–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Wolff, J. (1994). Democratic voting and the mixed-motivation problem. Analysis, 54, 193–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Japan Society for the Promotion of ScienceTokyoJapan
  2. 2.Department of Evolutionary Studies of BiosystemsSchool of Advanced Sciences, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies)HayamaJapan
  3. 3.Center for Advanced Intelligence Project, RIKENTokyoJapan

Personalised recommendations