Advertisement

Synthese

, Volume 190, Issue 12, pp 2165–2193 | Cite as

Semantics, conceptual spaces, and the meeting of minds

Article

Abstract

We present an account of semantics that is not construed as a mapping of language to the world but rather as a mapping between individual meaning spaces. The meanings of linguistic entities are established via a “meeting of minds.” The concepts in the minds of communicating individuals are modeled as convex regions in conceptual spaces. We outline a mathematical framework, based on fixpoints in continuous mappings between conceptual spaces, that can be used to model such a semantics. If concepts are convex, it will in general be possible for interactors to agree on joint meaning even if they start out from different representational spaces. Language is discrete, while mental representations tend to be continuous—posing a seeming paradox. We show that the convexity assumption allows us to address this problem. Using examples, we further show that our approach helps explain the semantic processes involved in the composition of expressions.

Keywords

Conceptual spaces Fixpoint semantics Meeting of minds Compositionality Cognitive semantics 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bates, E. (Ed.) (1976) Language and context. The acquisition of pragmatics. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Berge C. (1997) Topological spaces. Dover, MineolaGoogle Scholar
  3. Boroditsky L. (2000) Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition 75(1): 1–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brinck I. (2004) The pragmatics of imperative and declarative pointing. Cognitive Science Quarterly 3: 255–272Google Scholar
  5. Brinck I., Gärdenfors P. (2003) Co-operation and communication in apes and humans. Mind and Language 18: 484–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brouwer L. E. J. (1910) Über ein eindeutige, stetige Transformation von Flächen in sich. Mathematische Annalen 69: 176–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cameron P., Hogkin J. G., Naimpally S. A. (1974) Nearness: A better approach to continuity and limits. The American Mathematical Monthly 81(7): 739–745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clark H. (1992) Arenas of language use. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  9. Clark H., Schaefer E.F. (1989) Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science 13: 259–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Croft W., Cruse D. A. (2004) Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dekker P., van Rooij R. (2000) Bi-directional optimality theory: An application of game theory. Journal of Semantics 17: 217–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Evans V. (2006) Lexical concepts, cognitive models and meaning-construction. Cognitive Linguistics 17: 491–534CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fauconnier G., Turner R. (1998) Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science 22(2): 133–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fitting M. (2002) Fixpoint semantics for logic programming: A survey. Theoretical Computer Science 278: 25–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Galantucci B. (2005) An experimental study of the emergence of human communication systems. Cognitive Science 29: 737–767CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gärdenfors, P. (1997). Does semantics need reality? In Does representation need reality? (pp. 113–120). Austrian Society of Cognitive Science Technical Report 97-01, Vienna.Google Scholar
  17. Gärdenfors P. (2000) Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  18. Gärdenfors P. (2003) How homo became sapiens: On the evolution of thinking. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  19. Gärdenfors, P. (2007). Representing actions and functional properties in conceptual spaces. In T. Ziemke, J. Zlatev, & R. M. Frank (Eds.), Body, language and mind: Embodiment (Vol. 1, pp. 167–195). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  20. Gärdenfors P., Osvath M. (2010) The evolution of anticipatory cognition as a precursor to symbolic communication. In: Larson R. et al (eds) Evolution of language: Biolinguistic approaches. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 103–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gärdenfors, P., & Warglien, M. (to appear a). The development of semantic space for pointing and verbal communication. In J. Hudson, U. Magnusson & C. Paradis (Eds.), Conceptual spaces and the construal of spatial meaning: Empirical evidence from human communication. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Gärdenfors, P., & Warglien, M. (to appear b). Using conceptual spaces to model actions and events, to appear in Journal of Semantics.Google Scholar
  23. Garrod S., Anderson A. (1987) Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic coordination. Cognition 27(2): 181–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldin-Meadow S. (2007) Pointing sets the stage for learning language and creating language. Child Development 78: 741–745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Grice, P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Peter Cole & Jerry L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Harnad S. (1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42: 335–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Heim, I. (1983). On the projection problem for presuppositions. In Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics (Vol. II, pp. 114–125). Stanford, CA.Google Scholar
  28. Holyoak K. J., Thagard P. (1996) Mental leaps. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  29. Hopfield J. J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States 79: 2554–2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Holmqvist, K. (1993). Implementing cognitive semantics. Lund University Cognitive Studies 17, Lund.Google Scholar
  31. Jackendoff R. (2002) Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  32. Jäger G. (2007) The evolution of convex categories. Linguistics and Philosophy 30: 551–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jäger, G. (2010). Natural color categories are convex sets. In Logic, language and meaning, LNCS 6042 (pp. 11–20). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  34. Jäger G., van Rooij R. (2007) Language structure: Psychological and social constraints. Synthese 159: 99–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Johannesson, M. (2002). Geometric models of similarity. Lund University Cognitive Studies 87, Lund.Google Scholar
  36. Kripke S. (1975) Outline of a theory of truth. Journal of Philosophy 72: 690–716CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lakoff G. (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lakoff G., Johnson M. (1980) Metaphors we live by. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  39. Langacker R. W. (1986) An introduction to cognitive grammar. Cognitive Science 10: 1–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar (Vol. 1). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Lewis D. (1969) Convention. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  42. Lewis D. (1970) General semantics. Synthese 22: 18–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lewis D. (1979) Scorekeeping in a language game. Journal of Philosophical Logic 8: 339–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Maunder C. R. F. (1980) Algebraic topology. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  45. Mervis C., Rosch E. (1981) Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology 32: 89–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nosofsky R. M. (1988) Similarity, frequency, and category representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 14: 54–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Okabe A., Boots B., Sugihara K. (1992) Spatial tessellations: Concepts and applications of Voronoi diagrams. John Wiley & Sons, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  48. Parikh P. (2000) Communication, meaning and interpretation. Linguistics and Philosophy 23: 185–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Parikh P. (2010) Language and equilibrium. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  50. Pickering M. J., Garrod S. (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 169–190Google Scholar
  51. Putnam H. (1975) The meaning of ’meaning’. In: Gunderson K. (Ed.) Language, mind and knowledge. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp 131–193Google Scholar
  52. Rosch E. (1975) Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 104: 192–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rosch E. (1978) Prototype classification and logical classification: The two systems. In: Scholnik E. (Ed.) New trends in cognitive representation: Challenges to Piagets theory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 73–86Google Scholar
  54. Schelling T. (1960) The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Pres, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  55. Selten R., Warglien M. (2007) The emergence of simple languages in an experimental coordination game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104(18): 7361–7366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Shepard R. N. (1987) Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237: 1317–1323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Skyrms B. (1998) Salience and symmetry-breaking in the evolution of convention. Law and Philosophy 17: 411–418Google Scholar
  58. Stalnaker R. (1979) Assertion. Syntax and Semantics 9: 315–332Google Scholar
  59. Tomasello M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  60. Tourangeau R., Sternberg R. J. (1982) Understanding and appreciating metaphors. Cognition 11: 203–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. van Benthem J. (2008) Games that make sense: Logic, language and multi-agent interaction. In: Apt K., van Rooij R. (eds) New perspectives on games and interaction. Amsterdam University Press, Amsterdam, pp 197–209Google Scholar
  62. Varzi, A., & Warglien, M. (manuscript). Indeterminate contracts and semantic indeterminacy.Google Scholar
  63. Warglien, M. (2001). Playing conversation games. Paper presented at the 2001 Wittgenstein society symposium, Kirchberg.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Management, Advanced School of EconomicsUniversità Ca’ Foscari VeneziaVeneziaItaly
  2. 2.Laboratory for Experimental Economics / Fondazione Ca’ FoscariVeneziaItaly
  3. 3.Department of PhilosophyLundSweden

Personalised recommendations