Synthese

, Volume 175, Issue 2, pp 193–218 | Cite as

Darwinian ‘blind’ hypothesis formation revisited

Open Access
Article

Abstract

Over the last four decades arguments for and against the claim that creative hypothesis formation is based on Darwinian ‘blind’ variation have been put forward. This paper offers a new and systematic route through this long-lasting debate. It distinguishes between undirected, random, and unjustified variation, to prevent widespread confusions regarding the meaning of undirected variation. These misunderstandings concern Lamarckism, equiprobability, developmental constraints, and creative hypothesis formation. The paper then introduces and develops the standard critique that creative hypothesis formation is guided rather than blind, integrating developments from contemporary research on creativity. On that basis, I discuss three compatibility arguments that have been used to answer the critique. These arguments do not deny guided variation but insist that an important analogy exists nonetheless. These compatibility arguments all fail, even though they do so for different reasons: trivialisation, conceptual confusion, and lack of evidence respectively. Revisiting the debate in this manner not only allows us to see where exactly a ‘Darwinian’ account of creative hypothesis formation goes wrong, but also to see that the debate is not about factual issues, but about the interpretation of these factual issues in Darwinian terms.

Keywords

Darwinism Blind variation Creativity Hypothesis formation Guided variation Lamarckism Evolutionary epistemology Popper Campbell Simonton 

References

  1. Amundson R.A. (1989) The trials and tribulations of selectionist explanations. In: Hahlweg K., Hooker C. (eds) Issues in evolutionary epistemology. SUNY Press, Albany, pp 413–432Google Scholar
  2. Blute, M. (1979). Learning theory and the evolutionary analogy. Cogprints (1979), 2002. http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk.
  3. Boden M.A. (1998) Creativity and artificial intelligence. Artificial Intelligence, 103: 347–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boden M.A. (1999) Computer models of creativity. In: Sternberg R.J. (eds) Handbook of creativity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 351–391Google Scholar
  5. Boden M.A. (2004) The creative mind: Myths and mechanism (2nd ed). London and New York, RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
  6. Boyd R., Richerson P.J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  7. Briskman L. (1981) Creative product and creative process in science and art. In: Dutton D., Krausz M. (eds) The concept of creativity in science and art. Martinus Nijhoff Publ, Dordrecht, pp 129–155Google Scholar
  8. Buskes C. (1998) The genealogy of knowledge: A Darwinian approach to epistemology and philosophy of science. Tilburg University Press, TilburgGoogle Scholar
  9. Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400. (Reprinted in and quoted from Evolutionary epistemology, rationality, and the sociology of knowledge, by G. Radnitzky & W. W. Bartley, 1987 (pp. 91–114) LaSalle: Open Court).Google Scholar
  10. Campbell D.T. (1974a) Evolutionary epistemology. In: Schilpp P.A. (eds) The philosophy of Karl Popper Vol. 1. Open Court Publ, La Salle, pp 413–463Google Scholar
  11. Campbell D.T. (1974b) Unjustified variation and selective retention in scientific discovery. In: Ayala F., Dobszhansky T. (eds) Studies in the philosophy of biology: Reduction and related problems. Macmillan, London, pp 139–161Google Scholar
  12. Dasgupta S. (2004) Is creativity a Darwinian process? Creativity Research Journal, 16: 403–413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dawkins R. (1986) The blind watchmaker. Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  14. Findlay, A. (1968). A hundred years of chemistry (3rd rev. ed.). London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
  15. Fogel L.J. (1999) Intelligence through simulated evolution: Forty years of evolutionary programming. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. Futuyma D.J. (1998) Evolutionary biology (3rd ed). Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MAGoogle Scholar
  17. Gardner H. (1993) Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Gruber H.E. (1974) Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. Dutton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Gruber H.E., Davies S.N. (1988) Inching our way up Mount Olympus: The evolving-systems approach to creative thinking. In: Sternberg R.J. (eds) The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 243–270Google Scholar
  20. Gruber H.E., Wallace D.B. (1999) The case study method and evolving systems of approach for understanding unique creative people at work. In: Sternberg R.E. (eds) Handbook of creativity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 93–115Google Scholar
  21. Heyes, C., Hull, D.L. (eds) (2001) Selection theory and social construction. State University of New York Press, AlbanyGoogle Scholar
  22. Holland J.H. (1975) Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor, MIGoogle Scholar
  23. Hull D.L. (1988) Science as a process: An evolutionary account of the social and conceptual development of science. Chicago University Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  24. Hull D.L. (2001) Science and selection: Essays on biological evolution and the philosophy of science. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  25. Hull D.L., Glenn S.S., Langman R.E. (2001) A general account of selection: Biology, immunology and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24: 511–528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. James W. (1880) Great men and their environment. In: Burkhardt F.H., Bowers F., Skrupskelis I. (eds) The works of William James, Bd, 6: The will to believe and other essays in popular philosophy.. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 163–189Google Scholar
  27. Kim K. (2001) Nested hierarchies of vicarious selectors. In: Heyes C., Hull D.L. (eds) Selection theory and social construction. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 101–118Google Scholar
  28. Koza J.R. (1992) Genetic programming: On the programming of computers by means of natural selection. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  29. Kronfeldner M.E. (2007) Is cultural evolution Lamarckian? Biology and Philosophy, 22: 493–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kronfeldner, M. E. (forthcoming). Creativity naturalized. The Philosophical Quarterly.Google Scholar
  31. Luria S.E., Delbrück M. (1943) Mutations of bacteria from virus sensitivity to virus resistance. Genetics, 28: 491–511Google Scholar
  32. Mesoudi A. (2008) Foresight in cultural evolution. Biology and Philosophy, 23: 243–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mesoudi A., Whiten A., Laland K.N. (2006) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29: 329–383Google Scholar
  34. Mumford M.D. (1999) Blind variation or selective variation?: Evaluative elements in creative thought. Psychological Inquiry, 10: 344–348Google Scholar
  35. Mumford M.D., Antes A.L. (2007) Commentaries: Debates about the ‘general’ picture: Cognition and creative achievement. Creativity Research Journal, 19: 367–1374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nickles T. (2003) Evolutionary models of innovation and the Meno problem. In: Shavinina L.V. (eds) International handbook on innovation. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 54–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Poincaré, H. (1908). L’ invention mathématique. In H. Poincare (Ed.), Science et méthode (pp. 43–63). Paris: Flammurion. (Reprinted in and quotes from Foundations of science, 3rd ed., by H. Poincaré, 1982; G. B. Halsted, Trans., pp. 383–394, Washington: University Press of America).Google Scholar
  38. Popper K.R. (1972) Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  39. Popper K.R. (1974a) Intellectual autobiography. In: Schilpp P.A. (eds) The philosophy of Karl Popper, Bd.1. Open Court, LaSalle, pp 3–184Google Scholar
  40. Popper, K. R. (1974b). Campbell on the evolutionary theory of knowledge. In P. A. Schilpp (Ed.). The philosophy of Karl Popper (pp. 1059–65). LaSalle: Open Court. (Reprinted in and quoted from Evolutionary epistemology, rationality and the sociology of knowledge, pp. 115–120, by G. Radnitzky & W. W. Bartley, 1987, La Salle: Open Court).Google Scholar
  41. Popper, K. R. (1975). The rationality of scientific revolutions. In R. Harré (Ed.), Problems of scientific revolution (pp. 72–101). Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Reprinted in and quotes from Scientific revolutions, pp. 80–106, by J. Hacking, Ed., 1981, Oxford: Oxford UP).Google Scholar
  42. Popper K.R. (1984) Evolutionary Epistemology. In: Pollard J.W. (eds) Evolutionary theory: Paths into the future. Wiley, New York, pp 239–255Google Scholar
  43. Richards R.J. (1977) The natural selection model of conceptual evolution. Philosophy of Science, 44: 494–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Richerson P.J., Boyd R. (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  45. Roberts R.M. (1989) Serendipity: Accidental discoveries in science. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  46. Runco M.A. (2007) Creativity: Theories and themes, research, development, and practice. Elsevier, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  47. Ruse M. (1974) Cultural evolution. Theory and Decision, 5: 413–440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schaffer S. (1994) Making up discovery. In: Boden M.A. (eds) Dimensions of creativity. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 13–51Google Scholar
  49. Schooler J.W., Dougal S. (1999) Why creativity is not like the proverbial typing monkey. Psychological Inquiry, 10: 351–356Google Scholar
  50. Seifert C.M., Meyer D.E., Davidson N., Patalano A.L., Yaniv I. (1995) Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In: Sternberg R.J., Davidson J.E. (eds) The nature of insight. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 65–124Google Scholar
  51. Simon H.A. (1981) The Sciences of the Artificial, 2 ed. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  52. Simonton D.K. (1988) Scientific genius: A psychology of science. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  53. Simonton D.K. (1995) Foresight in insight? A Darwinian answer. In: Sternberg R.J., Davidson J.E. (eds) The nature of insight. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 465–494Google Scholar
  54. Simonton D.K. (1999a) Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  55. Simonton D.K. (1999b) Creativity as blind variation and selective retention: Is the creative process Darwinian. Psychological Inquiry, 10: 309–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Simonton D.K. (2003) Human creativity: Two Darwinian analyses. In: Reader S.M., Laland K.N. (eds) Animal innovation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 309–325Google Scholar
  57. Simonton D.K. (2005) Darwin as a straw man: Dasgupta’s (2004) Evaluation of creativity as a Darwinian process. Creativity Research Journal, 17: 299–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Simonton D.K. (2007a) Featured article: The creative process in Picasso’s Guernica sketches: Monotonic improvements versus nonmonotonic variants. Creativity Research Journal, 19: 329–344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Simonton D.K. (2007b) Rejoinder: Picasso’s Guernica: Creativity as a Darwinian process: Definitions, clarifications, misconceptions, and applications. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 381–394Google Scholar
  60. Skagestad P. (1978) Taking evolution seriously: Critical comments on D.T. Campbell’s evolutionary epistemology. The Monist, 61: 611–621Google Scholar
  61. Sniegowski P.D., Lenski R.E. (1995) Mutation and adaptation: The directed mutation controversy in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 26: 553–578CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sober E. (1992) Models of cultural evolution. In: Griffiths P. (eds) Trees of life: Essays in philosophy of biology. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 17–39Google Scholar
  63. Stein E., Lipton P. (1989) Where guesses come from: Evolutionary epistemology and the anomaly of guided variation. Biology and Philosophy, 4: 33–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Sternberg R.J. (1998) Cognitive mechanisms in human creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 32: 159–76Google Scholar
  65. Sternberg R.J. (2006) Cognitive psychology (4th ed). Belmont, ThompsonGoogle Scholar
  66. Sternberg, R.J. (eds) (1988) The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  67. Thagard P. (1988) Against evolutionary epistemology. In: Thagard P. (eds) Computational philosophy of science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 101–111Google Scholar
  68. Toulmin S. (1972) Human understanding: The collective use and evolution of concepts. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
  69. Ward T.B., Smith S.M., Finke R.A. (1999) Creative cognition. In: Sternberg R.J. (eds) Handbook of creativity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 189–212Google Scholar
  70. Weisberg R.W. (1993) Creativity: Beyond the myth of genius. Freeman and Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  71. Wimsatt W.C. (1999) Genes, memes and cultural heredity. Biology and Philosophy, 14: 279–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Max Planck Institute for the History of ScienceBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations