Springer Nature is making SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19 research free. View research | View latest news | Sign up for updates

Fear of the known? The effect of peer relevance and gender on women’s math performance under threat

Abstract

The goal of the current research was to investigate the impact of peer comparisons on female participants’ math performance when the peers varied on comparison relevance and gender. Specifically, the two studies presented here explored the question of whether male peers, who are seen as less relevant, can serve as peer role models (PRM) for female participants taking a math exam under stereotype threat conditions. Comparison relevance was manipulated by exposing participants to math-talented male and female peers who were from participants’ school or from another school. The key finding from Study1 demonstrated that exposure to relevant male peers lead to lower math performance compared to less relevant male peers. Study 2 replicated this key finding using a different manipulation of relevance. Together the findings indicate that when male peers are viewed as less relevant (and thus less threatening) they can serve as PRMs for female participants in “stereotyped” math situations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Notes

  1. 1.

    The schools used in Studies 1 and 2 were chosen because they were both public and similar in size to the participants’ school. Pretesting also demonstrated that participants had no preconceived notions about the type of students who attended those schools; hence, the students from these schools should not be particularly relevant to the participants in the current research.

  2. 2.

    Beyond the outcome measures described in the main text, participants also responded to a few additional questions and statements.

  3. 3.

    When these participants were included in the math performance analyses the interaction involving PRM relevance and gender was not significant, t(64) = 0.76, p > .45.

References

  1. Adams, G., Garcia, D. M., Purdie-Vaughns, V., & Steele, C. M. (2006). The detrimental effects of a suggestion of sexism in an instruction situation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,42, 602–615.

  2. Blanton, H., Buunk, B. P., Gibbons, F. X., & Kuyper, H. (1999). When better-than-others compare upward: Choice of comparison and comparative evaluation as independent predictors of academic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76, 420–430.

  3. Brickman, P., & Bulman, R. J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social comparison. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), In Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 149–186). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

  4. Buunk, B. P., & Ybema, J. F. (1997). Social comparison and occupational stress: The identification-contrast model. In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 359–388). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  5. Cash, T. F., Cash, D. W., & Butters, J. W. (1983). “Mirror, mirror, on the wall..?”: Contrast effects and self-evaluations of physical attractiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,9, 351–358.

  6. Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011). Understanding current causes of women’s underrepresentation in science. In PNAS: Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America (vol. 108, pp. 3157–3162).

  7. Chipman, S. F., Krantz, D. H., & Silver, R. (1992). Mathematics anxiety and science careers among able college women. Psychological Science, 3, 292–295.

  8. Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,60, 218–228.

  9. Crosnoe, R., Riegle-Crumb, C., Field, S., Frank, K., & Muller, C. (2008). Peer group contexts of adolescent academic experiences. Child Development,79, 139–155.

  10. Dijkstra, P., Kuyper, H., van der Werf, G., Buunk, A. P., & van der Zee, Y. G. (2008). Social comparison in the classroom: A review. Review of Educational Research,78, 828–879.

  11. Educational Testing Service [author]. (1994). GRE: Practicing to take the general test (9th edn.). Princeton.

  12. Educational Testing Service [author]. (2019). Overview of the quantitative reasoning measure. Retrieved from https://www.ets.org/gre/revised_general/prepare/quantitative_reasoning/.

  13. Erkut, S., & Mokros, J. R. (1984). Professors as models and mentors for college students. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 399–417

  14. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations,7, 117–140.

  15. Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Hochschild, L. (2002). When you and I are "we," you are not threatening: The role of self-expansion in social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82, 239–251.

  16. Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69, 227–236.

  17. Goethals, G. R., & Darley, J. (1977). Social comparison theory: An attributional approach. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical an empirical perspective (pp. 259–278). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

  18. Good, C., Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Why do women opt out? Sense of belonging and women's representation in mathematics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102, 700–717.

  19. Halisch, F., & Heckhausen, H. (1977). Search for feedback information and effort regulation during task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,35, 724–733.

  20. Hill, C., Corbett, C., & Rose, A. (2010). Why so few? Women in science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Washington, D.C.: AAUW.

  21. Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

  22. Huguet, P., Dumas, F., Monteil, J. M., & Genestoux, N. (2001). Social comparison choices in the classroom: Further evidence for students' upward comparison tendency and its beneficial impact on performance. European Journal of Social Psychology,31, 557–578.

  23. Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Ryan, C. (2008). Data analysis: A model comparison approach (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge Press.

  24. Ko, S. J., Marx, D. M., Nickerson, S. D., & Bjorkman, K. (2019). Implementing a peer role model program in college calculus classes to broaden women’s participation in STEM. PRIMUS: Problems, Resources, and Issues in Mathematics Undergraduate Studies.

  25. Lander, C. (2019). How similar is the GRE to the SAT? Retrieved from https://education.seattlepi.com/similar-gre-sat-1357.html.

  26. Levine, J. M. (1983). Social comparison and education. In J. M. Levine & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Teacher and student perceptions: Implications for learning (pp. 29–55). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  27. Lockwood, P. (2006). “Someone like me can be successful”: Do college students need same-gender role models? Psychology of Women Quarterly,30, 36–46.

  28. Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,73, 91–103.

  29. Logel, C., Walton, G. M., Spencer, S. J., Iserman, E. C., von Hippel, W., & Bell, A. E. (2009). Interacting with sexist men triggers social identity threat among female engineers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,96, 1089–1103.

  30. Major, B., Testa, M., & Blysma, W. H. (1991). Responses to upward and downward social comparisons: The impact of esteem-relevance and perceived control. In J. M. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 237–260). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  31. Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology,25, 431–467.

  32. Marx, D. M., & Goff, P. A. (2005). Clearing the air: The effect of experimenter race on targets’ test performance and subjective experience. British Journal of Social Psychology,44, 645–657.

  33. Marx, D. M., & Ko, S. J. (2012). Superstars “like” me: The effect of role model similarity on performance under threat. European Journal of Social Psychology,42, 807–812.

  34. Marx, D. M., Monroe, A. H., Cole, C. E., & Gilbert, P. N. (2013). No doubt about it: The effect of role model doubt on male’s and female’s math performance under threat. Journal of Social Psychology,153, 542–559.

  35. Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting female students math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,28, 1185–1197.

  36. Marx, D. M., Stapel, D. A., & Muller, D. (2005). We can do it: The interplay of a collective self-construal orientation and social comparisons under threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88, 432–446.

  37. McClelland, G. H. (2000). Nasty data: Unruly, ill-mannered observations can ruin your analysis. In H. T. Reis, C. M. Judd, H. T. Reis, & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 393–411). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  38. McClintock, C. G., & Van Avermaet, E. (1975). The effects of manipulating feedback upon children's motives and performance: A propositional statement and empirical evaluation. Behavioral Science,20, 101–116.

  39. Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., Vevea, J. L., Citkowicz, M., & Lauber, E. A. (2017). A re-examination of the mere exposure effect: The influence of repeated exposure on recognition, familiarity, and liking. Psychological Bulletin,143, 459–498.

  40. Muller, C., Riegle-Crumb, C., Schiller, K. S., Wilkinson, L., & Frank, K. A. (2010). Race and academic achievement in racially diverse high schools: Opportunity and stratification. Teachers College Record,112, 1038–1063.

  41. Muniz, H. (2017). GRE vs. SAT: 13 key differences. Retrieved from https://www.prepscholar.com/gre/blog/gre-vs-sat/.

  42. Mussweiler, T., Gabriel, S., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Shifting social identities as a strategy for deflecting threatening social comparisons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,79, 398–409.

  43. Pepitone, E. A. (1972). Comparison behavior in elementary school children. American Educational Research Journal,9, 45–63.

  44. Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women's generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues,57, 55–71.

  45. Reis, T. J., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F. X. (1993). Social comparison and the pill: Reactions to upward and downward comparison of contraceptive behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,19, 13–20.

  46. Riegle-Crumb, C. (2010). More girls go to college: Exploring the social and academic factors behind the female postsecondary advantage among Hispanic and white students. Research in Higher Education,51, 573–593.

  47. Riegle-Crumb, C., & Callahan, R. (2009). Exploring the academic benefits of friendship ties for Latino boys and girls. Social Science Quarterly,90, 611–631.

  48. Riegle-Crumb, C., & King, B. (2010). Questioning a white male advantage in STEM: Examining disparities in college major. Educational Researcher,39, 656–664.

  49. Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: Imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,96, 949–966.

  50. Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,38, 194–201.

  51. Shaffer, E. S., Marx, D. M., & Prislin, R. (2013). Mind the gap: Framing of women’s success and representation in STEM affects women’s math performance under threat. Sex Roles, 68, 454–463.

  52. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,35, 4–28.

  53. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype vulnerability and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69, 797–811.

  54. Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 379–440). San Diego: Academic Press.

  55. Taylor, S. E., & Lobel, M. L. (1989). Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychological Review,96, 569–575.

  56. Thagard, P., & Kunda, Z. (1998). Making sense of people: Coherence mechanisms. In S. J. Read & L. C. Miller (Eds.), Connectionist models of social reasoning and social behavior (pp. 3–26). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

  57. The College Board. (2011). College bound seniors national profile reports of SAT program test takers.

  58. The Princeton Review [author]. (2019). GRE sections. Retrieved from https://www.princetonreview.com/grad/gre-sections.

  59. Van Loo, K. J., Boucher, K. L., Rydell, R. T., & Rydell, M. T. (2013). Competition in stereotyped domains: Competition, social comparison, and stereotype threat. European Journal of Social Psychology,43, 648–660.

  60. Walton, G. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). Latent ability: Grades and test scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students. Psychological Science,20, 1132–1139.

  61. Wheeler, L., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). The proxy model of social comparison for self-assessment of ability. Personality and Social Psychology Review,1, 54–61.

  62. Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin,106, 231–248.

  63. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). The attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Writing of this article was supported by a Grant from the National Science Foundation (DRL# 1535117). I thank Krystal Carrillo, Danielle Palomar, Marie Ross, Stephanie van Stralen, Brad Weisz, Jessica Winet, and Robert Wyatt for their help with data collection and to Vito da Rosa and David Tobias Vargas for their feedback on earlier versions of this article. This article is dedicated to Cooper.

Author information

Correspondence to David M. Marx.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that there are no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Informed consent

These studies were approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at San Diego State University and all participants provided their consent prior to participation.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Marx, D.M. Fear of the known? The effect of peer relevance and gender on women’s math performance under threat. Soc Psychol Educ 22, 1197–1214 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-019-09519-0

Download citation

Keywords

  • Performance
  • Peers
  • Gender stereotypes
  • Social comparison
  • Relevance