Advertisement

Social Psychology of Education

, Volume 19, Issue 1, pp 23–39 | Cite as

What can boys and girls do? Preschoolers’ perspectives regarding gender roles across domains of behavior

  • Erin R. Baker
  • Marie S. Tisak
  • John Tisak
Article

Abstract

Previous research has examined at what age and in what contexts males and females develop gender-congruent stereotypes. Research indicates that social experience may provide a great influence on the presence of such stereotypes, but this is likely influenced by the development of gender schemas. The current study interviewed 99 children (3–6.5 years) in a sub-rural Midwestern university community. Females (N = 51, M age = 4.6, SD = 0.73) and males (N = 48, M age = 4.6, SD = 0.82) were individually asked who—boys, girls, or both—can do particular (1) occupations, (2) activities, (3) aggressive behaviors, and (4) prosocial behaviors. Generally, males tended to express holding no stereotypic beliefs, indicating gender-congruent expectations for only 2 items in one of the domains; however, females expressed multiple stereotypic beliefs for each of the four contexts. Social and cognitive explanations for these phenomena are discussed. The current study is an important addition to the existing literature in that preschool teachers and parents alike might be able to assist children to better develop activities and behavioral habits such that gender-related stereotypes fail to develop.

Keywords

Gender stereotypes Preschool gender development Social stereotypes 

References

  1. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bergin, C., Talley, S., & Hamer, L. (2002). Prosocial behaviors of young adolescents: A focus group study. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 13–32. doi: 10.1016/S0140-1971(02)00112-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Block, J. H. (1983). Differential premises arising from differential socialization of the sexes: Some conjectures. Child Development, 54, 1335–1354. doi: 10.2307/1129799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory of gender development and functioning. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Psychology of gender (pp. 92–119). New York: The Guilford Press. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.1064.676.Google Scholar
  6. Cameron, E., Eisenberg, N., & Tryon, K. (1985). The relations between sex-typed play and preschoolers’ social behavior. Sex Roles, 12, 602–615. doi: 10.1007/BF00288180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carlo, G. (2006). Care-based and altruistically based morality. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 551–579). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  8. Carlo, G., Hausmann, A., Christiansen, S., & Randall, B. A. (2003). Social cognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of prosocial tendencies for adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 107–134. doi: 10.1177/0272431602239132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chang, A., Sandhofer, C. M., & Brown, C. S. (2011). Gender biases in early number exposure to preschool aged children. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 30, 440–450. doi: 10.1177/0261927X11416207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differences in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: How do I hurt thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003–1014. doi: 10.2307/1131876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Foster, S. L., Delawyer, D. D., & Guevremont, D. C. (1986). A critical incidents analysis of liked and disliked peer behaviors and their situational parameters in childhood and adolescence. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 115–133.Google Scholar
  12. Freeman, N. K. (2007). Preschoolers’ perceptions of gender appropriate toys and their parents’ beliefs about genderized behaviors: Miscommunication, mixed messages, or hidden truths? Early Childhood Education Journal, 34, 357–366. doi: 10.1007/s10643-006-0123-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Goldstein, S., Tisak, M., & Boxer, P. (2002). Preschoolers’ normative and prescriptive judgments about relational and overt aggression. Early Education and Development, 13, 23–39. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1301_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Grusec, J. E. (2006). The development of moral behavior and conscience from a socialization perspective. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 243–265). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  15. Halim, M., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Zosuls, K., Lurye, L., & Greulich, F. (2014). Pink frilly dresses and the avoidance of all things “girly”: Children’s appearance rigidity and cognitive theories of gender development. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1091–1101. doi: 10.1037/a0034906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hastings, P., McShane, K., Parker, R., & Ladha, F. (2007a). Ready to make nice: Parental socialization of young sons’ and daughters’ prosocial behaviors with peers. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 168, 177–200. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.168.2.177-200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hastings, P. D., Utendale, W. T., & Sullivan, C. (2007b). The socialization of prosocial development. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp. 638–664). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  18. Hawley, P. H. (2003). Strategies of control, aggression, and morality in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. Journal Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 213–235. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00073-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hay, D. (1994). Prosocial development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 29–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01132.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hofferth, S., & Sandberg, J. (2001). How American children spend their time. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 295–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00295.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jackson, M., & Tisak, M. (2001). Is prosocial behavior a good thing? Developmental changes in children’s evaluations of helping, sharing, cooperating, and comforting. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 349–367. doi: 10.13458/026151001166146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Levy, G., Sandovsky, A., & Troseth, G. (2000). Aspects of young children’s perceptions of gender-typed occupations. Sex Roles, 42, 993–1006. doi: 10.1023/A:1007084516910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Martin, C. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with nontraditional and traditional gender roles. Sex Roles, 22, 151–165. doi: 10.1007/BF00288188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Martin, C., Ruble, D., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.6.903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Moon, M., & Hoffman, C. D. (2008). Mothers’ and fathers’ differential expectancies and behaviors: Parent X child gender effects. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 261–279. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.169.3.261-280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49, 400–407. doi: 10.2307/1128704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ostrov, J., Crick, N., & Keating, C. (2005). Gender biased perceptions of preschooler behavior: How much is aggression and prosocial behavior in the eye of the beholder? Sex Roles, 52, 393–398. doi: 10.1007/s11199-005-2681-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ostrov, J., & Keating, C. (2004). Gender differences in preschool aggression during free play and structured interactions: An observational study. Social Development, 13, 255–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.000266.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77, 847–860. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00906.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Persson, G. E. B. (2005). Developmental perspectives on prosocial and aggressive motives in preschoolers’ peer interactions. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 80–91. doi: 10.1080/01650250444000423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pomerantz, E. M., Ng, F. F.-Y., & Wang, Q. (2004). Gender socialization: A parent X child model. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Psychology of gender (pp. 120–144). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  32. Powlishta, K. K. (1995). Intergroup processes in childhood: Social categorization and sex role development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 781–788. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281. http://pwq.sagepub.com/content/26/4/269.full.pdf
  34. Raag, T., & Rackliff, C. (1998). Preschoolers’ awareness of social expectations of gender: Relationships to toy choices. Sex Roles, 38, 685–700. doi: 10.1023/A:1018890728636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Russell, A., & Owens, L. (1999). Peer estimates of school-aged boys’ and girl’s aggression to same and cross-sex targets. Social Development, 8, 364–379. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Spinrad, T. L., Losoya, S. H., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., et al. (1999). The relations of parental affect and encouragement to children’s moral emotions and behaviour. Journal of Moral Education, 28, 323–337. doi: 10.1080/030572499103115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Stoddart, T., & Turiel, E. (1985). Children’s concepts of cross-gender activities. Child Development, 56, 1241–1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tamis-LeMonda, C., & McFadden, K. (2009). The United States of America. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 299–322). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  39. Taylor, M. G. (1996). The development of children’s beliefs about social and biological aspects of gender differences. Child Development, 67, 1555–1571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tisak, M. S., & Block, J. H. (1990). Preschool children’s evolving conceptions of badness: A Longitudinal study. Early Education and Development, 1, 300–307. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed0104_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tisak, M. S., Holub, S. C., & Tisak, J. (2007). What nice things do boys and girls do? Preschoolers’ perspective of peers’ behaviors at and at home. Early Education and Development, 18, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/10409280701282686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tisak, M. S., Nucci, L. P., & Jankowski, A. M. (1996). Preschool children’s social interactions involving moral and prudential transgressions: An observational study. Early Education and Development, 7, 138–148. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed0702_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tisak, M. S., Tisak, J., & Goldstein, S. E. (2001). How do young children misbehave in the grocery store and in the school? The preschoolers’ perspective. Early Education and Development, 12, 487–498. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1204_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tisak, M., Tisak, J., & Laurene, K. (2012). Children’s judgments of social interactive behaviors with peers: The influence of age and gender. Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal, 15, 555–570. doi: 10.1007/s11218-012-9194-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Turiel, E. (1978). The development of concepts of social structure: Social convention. In J. Glick & K. A. Clarke-Stewart (Eds.), The development of social understanding (pp. 25–107). New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
  46. U.S. Census Bureau. (2014). The 2012 statistical abstract: Labor force, employment, and earnings. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/cats/labor_force_employment_earnings.html
  47. U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2014). News release: American time use survey2013 results. Retrieved from: http://www.bls.gov/news.release/atus.toc.htm
  48. Walker, S. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between young children’s peer-related social competence and individual differences in theory of mind. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 297–312. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.166.3.297-312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Witt, S. D. (2000). The influence of peers on children’s socialization to gender roles. Early Child Development and Care, 162, 1–7. doi: 10.1080/0300443001620101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Zosuls, K., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Shrout, P., Bornstein, M., & Greulich, F. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701. doi: 10.1037/a0014053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Bowling Green State UniversityBowling GreenUSA

Personalised recommendations