Fair is Fine, but More is Better: Limits to Inequity Aversion in the Domestic Dog
- 794 Downloads
- 26 Citations
Abstract
Research with domestic dogs provides a unique approach for exploring the evolution of fairness and justice. Not only are dogs descended from highly social canids; they have also been bred for cooperative tasks with humans. Dogs act cooperatively in social play and are skilled on other social cognitive tasks. It is reasonable to ask whether dogs behave in ways similar to primates in other social contexts. In particular, do dogs perceive and respond to unfairness or injustice, a skill potentially borne of long-term affiliation with and selection by humans? Using a revised test of inequity aversion which looks at advantageous and disadvantageous inequity, the current research investigated the behavior of 38 domestic dogs. Subject dogs and a control dog approached two trainers in turn: one who rewarded them equally for sitting on command and one who rewarded them unequally—either over-rewarding or under-rewarding the control dog. After familiarization with the trainers, subjects chose which trainer to approach by themselves. Subjects preferred the over-rewarding trainer over the fair trainer; they had no preference between the under-rewarding and the fair trainer. Further analyses found that length of ownership, subjects’ age, and cooperative work experience reversed the approach preference, predicting preference for the fair trainer—though breed did not. These results suggest that the precursory sensitivity, which dogs showed to iniquitous outcomes in prior research, does not extend to both advantageous and disadvantageous inequity and does not hold when the subject is continually rewarded. Dogs selected a trainer who had treated them “unfairly,” yet who presented a potentially greater opportunity for future rewards. When the stakes were high, dogs showed a greater sensitivity to the quantity of a reward than to the fairness of a reward.
Keywords
Fairness Domestic dog Inequity aversion Experimental designNotes
Acknowledgments
This study was approved by Columbia University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol AC-AAAC2044). Thanks are due to those who were integral to the running of the trials: Julie Hecht, Emily Cherenack, Adam Chapman, Orellana del Fierro, Rebekka Dohme, Rebecca Johnson, Meredith Leeman, Jennifer Oh, Shoshana Schoenfeld, Hannah Solomon, and Ilana Yablonovich. Drs. Heather Barry Kappes and Tom Tyler, previously at New York University, initiated this research and provided the conceptual foundation; Heather additionally provided great statistical support. Many thanks to Animal Haven, a non-profit shelter in New York City that generously donated use of their facility for running trials. The reviewers of this manuscript improved it, for which I give my thanks.
References
- Bekoff, M. (2002). Virtuous nature. New Scientist, 175, 34.Google Scholar
- Bekoff, M. (2004). Wild justice and fair play: Cooperation, forgiveness, and morality in animals. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 489–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bekoff, M., & Allen, C. (1998). Intentional communication and social play: How and why animals negotiate and agree to play. In M. Bekoff & J. A. Byers (Eds.), Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives (pp. 97–114). Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Blake, P. R., & McAuliffe, K. (2011). “I had so much it didn’t seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. Cognition, 120, 215–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175–181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bräuer, J., & Hanus, D. (2012). Social Justice Research, 25(3) (forthcoming).Google Scholar
- Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Social Justice Research, 19, 153–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009). Cebus apella tolerate intermittent unreliability in human experimenters. International Journal of Primatology, 30, 663–674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brosnan, S. F., Schiff, H. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 253–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brosnan, S. F., Talbot, C., Ahlgren, M., Lambeth, S. P., & Schapiro, S. J. (2010). Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1229–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 257–263.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johnson, T., McElreath, R., & Smirnov, O. (2007). Egalitarian motives in humans. Nature, 446, 794–796.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in human and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- de Waal, F. B. M., Leimgruber, K., & Greenberg, A. R. (2008). Giving is self-rewarding for monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 13685–13689.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817–868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gácsi, M., McGreevy, P., Kara, E., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). Effects of selection for cooperation and attention in dogs. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 5, 31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A., Varga, O., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2004). Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention. Animal Cognition, 7, 144–153.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298, 1634–1636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hauser, M. D., Cominsa, J. A., Pytkaa, L. M., Cahilla, D. P., & Velez-Calderona, S. (2011). What experimental experience affects dogs’ comprehension of human communicative actions? Behavioural Processes, 86, 7–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Horner, V., Carter, J. D., Suchak, M., & de Waal, F. (2011). Spontaneous prosocial choice by chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 13847–13851.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Horowitz, A. (2009). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Animal Cognition, 12, 107–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Horowitz, A. C., & Bekoff, M. (2007). Naturalizing anthropomorphism: Behavioral prompts to our humanizing of animals. Anthrozoös, 20, 23–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jensen, K., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). What’s in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 273, 1013–1021.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kundey, S. M. A., De Los Reyes, A., Royer, E., Molina, S., Monnier, B., German, R., et al. (2010). Reputation-like inference in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 14, 291–302.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Leonardi, R. J., Vick, S.-J., & Dufour, V. (2012). Waiting for more: The performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks. Animal Cognition, 15, 107–120.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 426–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lorenz, K. (1954). Man meets dog. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
- Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Ferrario, A., Valsecchi, P., & Prato-Previde, E. (2011). Social eavesdropping in the domestic dog. Animal Behaviour, 81, 1177–1183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1, 113–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Morell, V. (2009). Going to the dogs. Science, 325, 1062–1065.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Morris, P. H., Doe, C., & Godsell, E. (2008). Secondary emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports and subjective claims by animal owners. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Price, S. A., & Brosnan, S. F. (2012). To each according to his need? Variability in the responses to inequity in non-human primates. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi: 10.1007/s11211-012-0153-z.
- Pritchard, D., Dunnette, M. D., & Jorgenson, D. O. (1972). Effects of perceptions of equity and inequity on worker performance and satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 75–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Range, F., Horn, L., Viranyi, Z., & Huber, L. (2008). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 340–345.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Range, F., Leitner, K., & Virányi, Z. (2012). The influence of the relationship and motivation on inequity aversion in dogs. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi: 10.1007/s11211-012-0155-x.
- Reddy, V. (2010). Green eyes in bio-cultural frames. In S. Hart & M. Legerstee (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy: Theory, research and multidisciplinary approaches. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
- Russell, Y. I., Call, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Image scoring in great apes. Behavioural Processes, 78, 108–111.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1989). Envy and jealousy in close relationships. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 221–246.Google Scholar
- Schwab, C., & Huber, L. (2006). Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 169–175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Silk, J., Brosnan, S. F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D. J., Shapiro, S., Richardson, A., Lambeth, S. P., & Mascaro, J. (2005). Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 437, 1357–1359.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2002). Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 27–34.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Subiaul, F., Vonk, J., Okamoto-Barth, S., & Barth, J. (2008). Do chimpanzees learn reputation by observation? Evidence from direct and indirect experience with giving and withholding strangers. Animal Cognition, 11, 611–623.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (1997). Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behaviour in the dog. Anthrozoös, 10, 214–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tyler, T. R. (2001). Procedural strategies for gaining deference: Increasing social harmony or creating false consciousness? In J. M. Darley, D. M. Messick, & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Social influences on ethical behavior in organizations (pp. 69–87). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Udell, M. A. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). Ontogeny and phylogeny: Both are essential to human-sensitive behavior in the genus Canis. Animal Behaviour, 79, e9–e14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- West, R. E., & Young, R. J. (2002). Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? Animal Cognition, 5, 183–186.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). Fair refusal by capuchin monkeys. Nature, 428, 140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yamamoto, S., & Takimoto, A. (2012). Social Justice Research, 25(3) (forthcoming).Google Scholar