Social Justice Research

, Volume 25, Issue 2, pp 170–194 | Cite as

The Influence of the Relationship and Motivation on Inequity Aversion in Dogs

  • Friederike Range
  • Karin Leitner
  • Zsófia Virányi
Article

Abstract

One crucial element for the evolution of cooperation may be the sensitivity to others’ efforts and pay-offs in comparison with one’s own costs and gains. If an individual responds to a disadvantageous reward distribution, then it would likely increase its relative fitness compared with those who do not (Brosnan & de Waal, Nature 428:140, 2004). Recent experimental research indicates that sensitivity toward unequal reward distribution is not a uniquely human trait; non-human primates react to inequity if they witness a conspecific that obtains a more attractive reward for the same effort. However, primates are not the only species that cooperate and thus would benefit from inequity aversion. Canids also frequently cooperate with group members (e.g., wolves, wild dogs, etc.) or humans (domestic dogs), allowing for the possibility that they also are sensitive to the reward distribution. We report the findings of two studies of dogs’ responses to inequity and the social factors that mediate such responses. In the first study, we investigated whether domestic dogs showed a response to the inequity of rewards received for the same action in pairs of familiar dogs. We found that dogs showed significant behavioral differences when tested without food rewards when in the presence of a rewarded partner compared to the baseline and asocial control situations. This indicated that the presence of a rewarded partner matters (Range, Horn, Virányi, & Huber, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(1):340–345, 2009). In contrast to primates, dogs did not react to differences in the quality of food or effort. In the second study, we analyzed whether individual (motivational or personality) characteristics determined the response of each subject to unequal reward distributions or whether the subject’s responses depended on the specific relationship they had with its partner. We found that individual motivation and the quality of the relationship influenced the dogs’ performance in test conditions, but that these factors varied across the control and reward inequity conditions. Overall, our results suggest that inequity aversion in dogs is conditional on their and their partner’s rewards. Whether inequity aversion is based on the same mechanisms in both humans and non-human primates is still unclear.

Keywords

Inequity Motivation Canis familiaris Relationship quality 

References

  1. Alberts, S. C. (1994). Vigilance in young baboons: Effects of habitat, age, sex, and maternal rank on glance rate. Animal Behaviour, 47, 749–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amsel, A. (1992). Frustration theory: Many years later. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 396–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amsel, A., & Roussel, J. (1952). Motivational properties of frustration: I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 363–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bekoff, M. (2004). Wild justice, cooperation and fair play: Minding manners, being nice, and feeling good. In R. Sussman & A. Chapman (Eds.), The origins and nature of sociality (pp. 53–79). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
  5. Benus, R. F., den Daas, S., Koolhaas, J. M., & Oortmerssen, G. A. (1990). Routine formation and flexibility in social and non-social behaviour of aggressive and non-aggressive male mice. Behaviour, 112(3–4), 176–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bernstein, D. I. S., Judge, P. G., & Ruehlmann, T. E. (1993). Kinship, association, and social relationships in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 31(1), 41–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boitani, L., & Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 7(1), 49–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2010a). Effect of group size, dominance rank and social bonding on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Animal Behavior, 79, 981–991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bonanni, R., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2010b). Pattern of individual participation and cheating in conflicts between groups of free-ranging dogs. Animal Behavior, 79, 957–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 3123–3128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175–181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bräuer, J., & Hanus, D. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming). Google Scholar
  13. Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Journal of Social Justice, 19, 153–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brosnan, S. F. (2011). A hypothesis of the co-evolution of cooperation and responses to inequity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425(6955), 297–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2004). Fair refusal by capuchin monkeys. Nature, 428, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Partial support from a nonreplication: Comment on Roma, Silberberg, Ruggiero, and Suomi. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 74–75.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Brosnan, S. F., Freeman, C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Partner’s behavior, not reward distribution, determines success in an unequal cooperative task in capuchin monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 68(7), 713–724.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Brosnan, S. F., Schiff, H. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 1560, 253–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Brosnan, S. F., Talbot, C., Ahlgren, M., Lambeth, S. P., & Schapiro, S. J. (2010). Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behavior, 79, 1229–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Butler, J. R. A., du Toit, J. T., & Bingham, J. (2004). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: Threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores. Biological Conservation, 115(3), 369–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R., & Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 443–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Caine, N. G., & Marra, S. L. (1988). Vigilance and social organization in two species of primates. Animal Behavior, 36, 897–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Capitanio, J. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: Prediction of behaviors across time and situation. American Journal of Primatology, 47, 299–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Carder, B., & Berkowitz, K. (1970). Rats’ preference for earned in comparison with free food. Science, 167, 1273–1274.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Clark, M. S., & Grote, N. K. (2003). Close relationships. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 447–461). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  27. Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origins of the dogs: Domestication and early history. In J. A. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 199–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Coppinger, R., & Schneider, R. (1995). Evolution of working dogs. In J. A. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 21–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Courchamp, F., & Macdonald, D. W. (2001). Crucial importance of pack size in the African wild dog Lycaon pictus. Animal Conservation, 4, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Creel, S., & Creel, N. M. (1995). Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Animal Behavior, 50, 1325–1339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Creel, S., Creel, N. M., Mills, M. G. L., & Monfort, S. L. (1997). Rank and reproduction in cooperatively breeding African wild dogs: Behavioral and endocrine correlates. Behavioral Ecology, 8(3), 298–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Daniels, T. J., & Bekoff, M. (1989). Population and social biology of free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Journal of Mammalogy, 70, 754–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Day, R. L., Coe, R. L., Kendal, J. R., & Laland, K. N. (2003). Neophilia, innovation and social learning: A study of intergeneric differences in callitrichid monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 65, 559–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. de Waal, F. B. M., & Luttrell, L. M. (1988). Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethology and Sociobiology, 9(2–4), 101–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Drent, P. J., & Marchetti, C. (1999). Individuality, exploration and foraging in hand-raised juvenile great tits. In N. J. Adams & R. H. Slotow (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd international ornithological congress, Durban (pp. 896–914).Google Scholar
  36. Dubreuil, D., Gentile, M. S., & Visalberghi, E. (2006). Are capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 1223–1228.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Dugatkin, L. A. (Ed.). (1997). Cooperation among animals. Oxford series in ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Erdohegyi, A., Topal, J., Virányi, Z., & Miklosi, A. (2007). Dog-logic: Inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task and its restricted use. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 725–737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422(6928), 137–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Fontenot, M. B., Watson, S. L., Roberts, K. A., & Miller, R. W. (2007). Effects of food preferences on token exchange and behavioural responses to inequality in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 74, 487–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Freeman, H. D., & Gosling, S. D. (2010). Personality in nonhuman primates: A review and evaluation of past research. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 1–19.Google Scholar
  43. Gosling, S. D. (1998). Personality dimensions in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(2), 107–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2003). A dog’s got personality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgements in dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1161–1169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Harrington, F. H., Mech, L. D., & Fritts, S. H. (1983). Pack size and wolf pup survival—Their relationship under varying ecological conditions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 13(1), 19–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Heinrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., et al. (2001). In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91, 73–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2008). Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 835–840.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Horowitz, A. (2012). Fair is fine, but more is better: Limits to inequity aversion in the domestic dog. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0158-7.
  50. Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van der Wegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: Current status in behaviour and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 925–935.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 426–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Lorenz, K. (1974). Analogy as a source of knowledge. Science, 185, 229–234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Massen, J. J. M., van den Berg, L. M., Spruijt, B. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2011). Inequity aversion and the effect of relationship quality in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology. doi:10.1002/ajp.21014.
  54. Mech, D. (1970). The wolf: The ecology and behaviour of an endangered species. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press.Google Scholar
  55. Mech, L. D. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie, 77(8), 1196–1203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Mech, L. D., & Boitani, L. (2003). Wolf social ecology. In L. D. Mech & L. Boitani (Eds.), Wolves: Behavior, ecology, and conservation (pp. 1–35). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  57. Miklosi, A., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: What can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour, 67, 995–1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. R. (2009). Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behavioural Processes, 80, 109–114.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Naderi, S., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V. (2001). Co-operative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 74, 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Pal, S. K. (2005). Parental care in free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90(1), 31–47. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B., & Roy, S. (1998). Agonistic behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to season, sex and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 59(4), 331–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Pang, J.-F., Kluetsch, C., Zou, X.-J., Zhang, A.-b., Luo, L.-Y., Angleby, H., et al. (2009). mtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of Yangtze River, less than 16,300 years ago, from numerous wolves. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 26(12), 2849–2864.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Papini, M. R. (2003). Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 62, 83–95.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Papini, M. R., & Dudley, R. T. (1997). Consequences of surprising reward omissions. Review of General Psychology, 1, 175–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Pierce, J., & Bekoff, M. (2012). Wild justice redux: What we know about social justice in animals and why it matters. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0154-y.
  66. Pongrácz, P., Vida, V., Bánhegyi, P., & Miklósi, Á. (2008). How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? Animal Cognition, 11, 75–82.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Price, S. A., & Brosnan, S. F. (2012). To each according to his need? Variability in the responses to inequity in non-human primates. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0153-z.
  68. Raihani, N. J., & McAuliffe, K. (2012). Does inequity aversion motivate punishment? Cleaner fish as a model system. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0157-8.
  69. Range, F., Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Gajdon, G. K., & Huber, L. (2009a). Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children. Animal Cognition, 12(1), 181–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Range, F., Horn, L., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L. (2009b). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(1), 340–345.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2011). Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus). PLoS ONE, 6(2), e16888. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016888.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Range, F., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L. (2007). Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Current Biology, 17(10), 868–872.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Roma, P. G., Silberberg, A., Ruggiero, A. M., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Capuchin monkeys, inequity aversion, and the frustration effect. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 67–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Savolainen, P., Zhang, Y. P., Luo, J., Lundeberg, J., & Leitner, T. (2002). Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1610–1613.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  76. Sherman, B. (1980). The formation of relationships based on socio-spatial proximity in a free-ranging colony of rhesus monkeys. Primates, 21(4), 484–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Sherman, S. M., & Wilson, J. R. (1975). Behavioral and morphological evidence for binocular competition in the postnatal development of the dog’s visual system. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 161, 183–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Stevens, J. R., & Gilby, I. C. (2004). A conceptual, framework for nonkin food sharing: timing and currency of benefits. Animal Behaviour, 67, 603–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Szetei, V., Miklosi, A., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (2003). When dogs seem to lose their nose: An investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 83(2), 141–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tinklepaugh, O. L. (1982). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 197–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Topal, J., Gergely, G., Erdohegyi, A., Csibra, G., & Miklosi, A. (2009). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science, 325(5945), 1269–1272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., & Wensing, J. A. B. (1987). Dominance and its behavioural measures in a captive wolf pack. In H. Frank (Ed.), Man and wolf (pp. 219–252). Dordrecht: Junk Publishers.Google Scholar
  83. van Wolkenten, M., Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Inequity responses of monkeys modified by effort. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 18854–18859.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Verbeek, M. E. M., Boon, A., & Drent, P. J. (1996). Exploration, aggressive behaviour and dominance in pair-wise confrontations of juvenile great tits. Behaviour, 133, 945–963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Virányi, Zs., & Range, F. (submitted). Wolves understanding of human communicative cues in a two choice task.Google Scholar
  86. Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). Fair refusal by capuchin monkeys. Nature, 428, 140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Yamamoto, S., & Takimoto, A. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming). Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Friederike Range
    • 1
    • 2
  • Karin Leitner
    • 3
  • Zsófia Virányi
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Messerli Research InstituteUniversity of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University of Vienna & University of ViennaViennaAustria
  2. 2.Wolf Science CenterErnstbrunnAustria
  3. 3.Department of Cognitive BiologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations