Advertisement

Social Indicators Research

, Volume 115, Issue 3, pp 1087–1099 | Cite as

The Hedonic and Eudaimonic Validity of the Orientations to Happiness Scale

  • Luke Wayne HendersonEmail author
  • Tess Knight
  • Ben Richardson
Article

Abstract

The orientations to happiness scale (OTH) was designed to measure three routes to happiness: pleasure (hedonia), meaning (eudaimonia) and engagement (flow). Past research utilising the scale suggests that all orientations predict life satisfaction, with meaning and engagement the stronger predictors relative to pleasure. However, these findings are inconsistent with other research; one plausible explanation being that the OTH scale lacks validity. This was tested by having participants (N = 107) complete the OTH scale and the Satisfaction with Life scale, prior to completing an online diary reporting actual instances of hedonic and eudaimonic behaviour. Although meaning predicted eudaimonic behaviour, the pleasure orientation was unrelated to hedonic behaviour. Further, hedonic behaviour was more strongly related to life satisfaction than eudaimonic behaviour; inconsistent with OTH scale results. These findings challenge the validity of the OTH scale, and subsequently bring into question those conclusions drawn from past research utilising the OTH scale.

Keywords

Eudaimonia Hedonia Happiness Validity Well-being Orientations Pathways 

References

  1. Aristotle. (1985). Nicomachean ethics (T. Irwin, Trans.). Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
  2. Australian Bureau of Statistics (2008). 2006 time use survey Canberra, Australian Capital Territory.Google Scholar
  3. Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., & King, L. A. (2009). Two traditions of happiness research, not two distinct types of happiness. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(3), 208–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chan, D. W. (2009). Orientations to happiness and subjective well-being among Chinese prospective and in-service teachers in Hong Kong. Educational Psychology, 29(2), 139–151. doi: 10.1080/01443410802570907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chen, L., Tsai, Y. M., & Chen, M. Y. (2010). Psychometric analysis of the orientations to happiness questionnaire in Taiwanese undergraduate students. Social Indicators Research, 98(2), 239–249. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9473-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
  7. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy. American Psychologist, 54(10), 821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). Good business: Leadership, flow, and the making of meaning. New York: Penguin Group.Google Scholar
  9. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Wissing, M. (2011). The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: Qualitative and quantitative findings. Social Indicators Research, 100(2), 185–207. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Delle, A., Massimini, F., & Bassi, M. (2011). Hedonism and eudaimonism in positive psychology. In A. Delle Fave (Ed.), Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures: Social empowerment through personal growth, Vol. 2 (pp. 3–18). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31(2), 103–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Diener, E. (2009). Subjective well-being. In E. Diener (Ed.), The science of well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener, Vol. 37 (pp. 11–58). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dockray, S., Grant, N., Stone, A. A., Kahneman, D., Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (2010). A comparison of affect ratings obtained with ecological momentary assessment and the day reconstruction method. Social Indicators Research, 99(2), 269–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frankl, V. E. (1988). The will to meaning: Foundations and applications of logotherapy. In V. E. Frankl (Ed.), Meridian. New York: Plume.Google Scholar
  17. Gosling, S. D., Jothn, O. P., Craik, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (1998). Do people know how they behave? Self-reported act frequencies compared with on-line codings by observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1337–1349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Grinde, B. (2012). The biology of happiness. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Henderson, L. W., & Knight, T. (2012). Integrating the hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives to more comprehensively understand wellbeing and pathways to wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 2(3), 196–221. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v2i3.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Huta, V. (2013). Eudaimonia. In S. David, I. Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Happiness, Chap. 14. Oxford, GB: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(6), 735–762. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 1776–1780. doi: 10.1126/science.1103572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kashdan, T. B., Biswas-Diener, R., & King, L. A. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: The costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 219–233. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Keyes, C. L., & Annas, J. (2009). Feeling good and functioning well: Distinctive concepts in ancient philosophy and contemporary science. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(3), 197–201. doi: 10.1080/17439760902844228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Linley, A. P., Joseph, S., Harrington, S., & Wood, A. M. (2006). Positive psychology: Past, present, and (possible) future. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(1), 3–16. doi: 10.1080/17439760500372796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Linley, A. P., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Osborne, G., & Hurling, R. (2009). Measuring happiness: The higher order factor structure of subjective and psychological well-being measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 878–884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Multinational Time Use Study. (2012). MTUS coding procedures. http://www.timeuse.org/mtus/data: Fisher, K.
  28. Park, N., Peterson, C., & Ruch, W. (2009). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction in twenty-seven nations. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(4), 273–279. doi: 10.1080/17439760902933690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6(1), 25–41. doi: 10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ruch, W., Harzer, C., Proyer, R. T., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2010). Ways to happiness in German-Speaking countries. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 26(3), 227–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ryan, R. M., & Huta, V. (2009). Wellness as healthy functioning or wellness as happiness: The importance of eudaimonic thinking (response to the Kashdan et al. and Waterman discussion). The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(3), 202–204. doi: 10.1080/17439760902844285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Schueller, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: Relationships to subjective and objective measures of well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4), 253–263. doi: 10.1080/17439761003794130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Vella-Brodrick, D., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2009). Three ways to be happy: Pleasure, engagement, and meaning—Findings from Australian and US samples. Social Indicators Research, 90(2), 165–179. doi: 10.1007/s11205-008-9251-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Vittersø, J., Dyrdal, G. M., & Røysamb, E. (2006). Why can’t we measure engagement with life satisfaction scales? Some theoretical and empirical arguments about their distinctiveness. Paper presented at the The 3rd European conference on positive psychology, Braga, Portugal.Google Scholar
  35. Vittersø, J., & Søholt, Y. (2011). Life satisfaction goes with pleasure and personal growth goes with interest: Further arguments for separating hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(4), 326–335. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2011.584548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 678–691. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: A eudaimonist’s perspective. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 234–252. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S., & Conti, R. (2008). The implications of two conceptions of happiness (hedonic enjoyment and eudaimonia) for the understanding of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 41–79. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9020-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Luke Wayne Henderson
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Tess Knight
    • 1
    • 2
  • Ben Richardson
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.School of PsychologyDeakin UniversityMelbourneAustralia
  2. 2.BurwoodAustralia

Personalised recommendations