Social Indicators Research

, Volume 110, Issue 2, pp 579–595 | Cite as

On the Sunny Side of Life: Sunshine Effects on Life Satisfaction

Article

Abstract

This paper seeks to analyze the influence of the weather on a person’s self-reported life satisfaction. On a theoretical level, it is claimed that ‘nice’ weather can improve the affective well-being of a person. Given this, it is argued that affects can, in turn, have an impact on that person’s general assessment of his or her life. In particular, it is expected that people would report a higher life satisfaction on days with unambiguously ‘nice’ weather. Data from three German large-scale surveys are used to test empirically to what extent self-reported life satisfaction is determined by the weather. All in all, the results are mostly consistent with the initial hypothesis. In all three samples those respondents surveyed on days with exceptionally sunny weather reported a higher life satisfaction compared to respondents interviewed on days with ‘ordinary’ weather. In two out of three samples, this difference was statistically significant. Hence, the supposed sunshine effect on peoples’ life satisfaction does indeed exist. Implications of these findings are discussed in a conclusion.

Keywords

Well-being Life satisfaction Happiness Mood Weather Sunshine Climate conditions 

References

  1. Barker, A., Hawton, K., Fagg, J., & Jennison, C. (1994). Seasonal and weather factors in Parasuicide. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 375–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Becchetti, L., Castriota, S., & Londono Bedoya, D. A. (2007). Climate, happiness and the kyoto protocol: Someone does not like it hot. CEIS Working Paper, No. 247. University of Rome Tor Vergata.Google Scholar
  3. Berkowitz, L. (1993). Pain and aggression: Some findings and implications. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 277–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 561–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Canbeyli, R. (2010). Sensorimotor modulation of mood and depression: An integrative review. Behavioural Brain Research, 207, 249–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 393–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen, A. (2011). The photosynthetic president: Converting sunshine into popularity. Social Science Journal, 48, 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Compton, R. J., Wirtz, D., Pajoumand, G., Claus, E., & Heller, W. (2004). Association between positive affect and attentional shifting. Cognitive Research and Therapy, 28, 733–744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Denissen, J. J. A., Butalid, L., Penke, L., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2008). The effects of weather on daily mood: A multilevel approach. Emotion, 8, 662–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Even, C., Schroder, C. M., Friedman, S., & Roullion, F. (2008). Efficacy of light therapy in nonseasonal depression: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 108, 11–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Forgas, J. (2008). Affect and cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 94–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frijters, P., & van Praag, B. M. S. (1998). The effects of climate on welfare and well-being in Russia. Climate Change, 39, 61–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Greifeneder, R., Bless, H., & Pham, M. T. (2010). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 1–35.Google Scholar
  16. Guven, C. (2009). Weather and financial risk-taking: is happiness the channel? SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research (Vol. 218). Berlin: DIW.Google Scholar
  17. Harmatz, M. G., Well, A. D., Overtree, C. E., Kawamura, K. Y., Rosal, M., & Ockene, I. S. (2000). Seasonal variation of depression and other moods: A longitudinal approach. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 15, 344–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Holland, R., Sayers, J., Keatinge, W., Davis, H., & Peswani, R. (1985). Effects of raised body temperature on reasoning, memory, and mood. The Journal of Applied Physiology, 59, 1823–1827.Google Scholar
  19. Isen, A. M. (2008). Some ways in which positive affect influences decision making and problem solving. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 548–573). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  20. Jacobsen, F. M., Wehr, T. A., Sack, D. A., James, S. P., & Rosenthal, N. E. (1987). Seasonal affective disorder. A review of the syndrome and its public health implications. American Journal of Public Health, 77, 57–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Johnson, E. M., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 20–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Keller, M. C., Fredrickson, B. L., Ybarra, O., Coté, S., Johnson, K., Mikels, J., et al. (2005). A warm heart and a clear head: The contingent effects of weather on mood and cognition. Psychological Science, 16, 724–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kripke, D. F. (1998). Light treatment for nonseasonal depression: Speed, efficacy, and combined treatment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49, 109–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kuppens, P., Realo, A., & Diener, E. (2008). The role of positive and negative emotions in life satisfaction judgment across nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 66–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Leppämäki, S., Partonen, T., & Lönnqvist, J. (2002). Bright-light exposure combined with physical exercise elevates mood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 139–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. MacLeod, C. (1999). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In T. Dalgeish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 447–477). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  27. MacLeod, C., & Campbell, L. (1992). Memory accessibility and probability judgments: An experimental evaluation of the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 890–902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Magnusson, A. (2000). An overview of epidemiological studies on seasonal affective disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 176–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 119–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A Cognitive-motivational analysis of Anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809–848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Murray, K. B., Di Muro, F., Finn, A., & Leczczyc, P. (2010). The effect of weather on consumer spending. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 17, 512–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mutz, M., & Kämpfer, S. (2011). …und nun zum Wetter: Beeinflusst die Wetterlage die Einschätzung von politischen und wirtschaftlichen Sachverhalten? [...and now the weather: Does weather influence the assessment of political and economic issues?] Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 40, 208–226.Google Scholar
  33. Oyane, N. M., Bjelland, I., Pallesen, S., Holsten, F., & Bjorvatn, B. (2008). Seasonality is associated with anxiety and depression: The Hordaland health study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 105, 147–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Page, L. A., Hajat, S., & Kovats, R. S. (2007). Relationship between daily suicide counts and temperature in England and Wales. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 106–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Parker, P. M., & Tavassoli, N. T. (2000). Homeostasis and consumer behavior across cultures. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 17, 33–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pilcher, J. J. (1998). Affective and daily event predictors of life satisfaction in college students. Social Indicators Research, 43, 291–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Przuntek, H., & Müller, T. (Eds.). (2005). Das serotonerge System aus neurologischer und psychiatrischer Sicht. Darmstadt: Steinkopff.Google Scholar
  38. Rehdanz, K., & Maddison, D. (2005). Climate and happiness. Ecological Economics, 52, 111–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Robinson, M. D. (2004). Personality as performance: Categorization tendencies and their correlates. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 127–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Robinson, M. D., & Compton, R. J. (2008). The happy mind in action: The cognitive basis of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 220–238). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  41. Rosenthal, N. E., Moul, D. E., Hellekson, C. J., Oren, D. A., Frank, A., Brainard, G. C., et al. (1993). A multicenter study of light visor for seasonal affective disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 8, 151–160.Google Scholar
  42. Rosenthal, N. E., Sack, D. A., Gillin, J. C., Lewy, A. J., Goodwin, F. K., Davenport, Y., et al. (1984). Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 72–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Schimmack, U. (2008). The structure of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 97–123). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  44. Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002a). Life satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70, 345–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2005). The influence of chronically and temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 395–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002b). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 582–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schwarz, N. (2011). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 289–308). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
  48. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgements of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1991). Evaluating one’s life: A judgment model of subjective well-being. In F. Strack & M. Argyke (Eds.), Subjective well-being. In interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 27–47). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  50. Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahnemann, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
  51. Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Kommer, D., & Wagner, D. (1987). Soccer, rooms, and the quality of life: Mood effects on judgments of satisfaction with life in general and with specific life domains. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 67–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 482–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tamir, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2007). The happy spotlight: Positive mood and selective attention to positive information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1124–1136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., & Solberg, E. C. (2006). You may worry, but can you recognize threats when you see them? Journal of Personality, 74, 1481–1506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. The Economist. (2005). The Economist Intelligence Unit′s quality-of-life index. Retrieved from http://www.economist.com/media/pdf/QUALITY_OF_LIFE.pdf.
  56. Tversky, A., & Griffin, D. (1991). Endowment and contrast in judgments of well-being. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being (pp. 101–118). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  57. Veenhoven, R. (2008). The international scale interval study. In V. Molle & D. Huschka (Eds.), Quality of life in the new millennium advances-in-quality studies, theoriy and research (pp. 45–58). Dordrecht: Springer Press Social Indicator Research Series.Google Scholar
  58. Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  59. Winkler, D., Willeit, M., Praschak-Rieder, N., Lucht, M. J., Hilger, E., Konstantinidis, A., et al. (2002). Changes of clinical pattern in seasonal affective disorder (SAD) over time in a german-speaking sample. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 252, 54–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Yardley, J. K., & Rice, R. W. (1991). The relationship between mood and subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24, 101–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute for SociologyFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations