Advertisement

Sex Roles

, Volume 79, Issue 5–6, pp 358–373 | Cite as

Parents’ Influence on Infants’ Gender-Typed Toy Preferences

  • Josh L. Boe
  • Rebecca J. Woods
Original Article

Abstract

Gender socialization influences children at early ages, shaping their developing identities. The toys provided by parents deliver some of the earliest gender-based messages by encouraging children to engage in activities associated with, for example, dolls and trucks. In the current study, we measured the influence of parental socialization by assessing 5- and 12 ½-month-old infants’ exposure to dolls and trucks and by experimentally manipulating parents’ encouragement to play with these toys. We found that infants displayed gender-typical toy preferences at 12 ½, but not 5 months, a pattern characteristic of previous studies. However, brief encouragement by a parent to play with toys from each category was ineffective in altering infants’ preferences. Rather, the types of toys present in the home predicted preferences, suggesting that at-home exposure to toys may be influential in the development of toy preferences. These findings reveal that socialization processes may indeed play a role in the formation of early gender-typical toy preferences and highlight the importance of equal toy exposure during infancy to ensure optimal development.

Keywords

Gender differences Gender Infant Parents Toys Gender socialization 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The present research was supported in part by an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under grant number P20 GM103505.

We would like to thank Caitlin Van Voorhis, Ellen Honsa, Savanna Westrom and the research assistants of the Infant Cognitive Development Lab at North Dakota State University for their help with data collection and manuscript preparation, as well as the parents who agreed to have their infants participate in this research.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards set forth by the American Psychological Association and by the home institution’s Institutional Review Board. Informed consent and parental permission was obtained for all participants. This manuscript was funded in part by the National Institutes of Health. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests relating to this research.

Supplementary material

11199_2017_858_MOESM1_ESM.docx (1 mb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 1033 kb)

References

  1. Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue, and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32(1), 7–14.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021833110722.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander, G. M. (2006). Associations among gender-linked toy preferences, spatial ability, and digit ratio: Evidence from eye-tracking analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35(6), 669–709.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9038-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Ceropithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–433.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00107-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Alexander, G. M., & Saenz, J. (2012). Early androgens, activity levels and toy choices of children in the second year of life. Hormones and Behavior, 62(4), 500–504.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.08.008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Alexander, G. M., Wilcox, T., & Woods, R. (2009). Sex differences in infants’ visual interest in toys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(3), 427–433.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9430-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Aslin, R. N. (2007). What’s in a look? Developmental Science, 10, 48–53.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00563.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Barry III, H., & Harper, A. S. (1995). Increased choice of female phonetic attributes in first names. Sex Roles, 32, 809–819.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.87.1.48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beall, A. E. (1993). A social constructionist view of gender. In A. E. Beall & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (pp. 127–147). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  9. Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3(3), 203–206.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00028.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Berthier, N. E., & Keen, R. (2006). Development of reaching in infancy. Experimental Brain Research, 169(4), 507–518.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0169-9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bhana, D., & Mayeza, E. (2016). We don’t play with gays, they’re not real boys…they can’t fight: Hegemonic masculinity (homophobic) violence in the primary years of schooling. International Journal of Educational Development, 51, 36–42.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2016.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Birke, L. I., & Vines, G. (1987, January). Beyond nature versus nurture: process and biology in the development of gender. Women’s Studies International Forum, 10(6), 555–570.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-5395(87)90070-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Blair, S. L. (1992). The sex-typing of children’s household labor: Parental influences on daughters’ and sons’ house work. Youth and Society, 24, 530–543.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X92024002004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Blakemore, J. E. O. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys. Sex Roles, 48, 411–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Blakemore, J. E. O., Berenbaum, S., & Liben, L. (2009). Gender development. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.Google Scholar
  16. Blakemore, J. E. O., & Centers, R. E. (2005). Characteristics of boys’ and girls’ toys. Sex Roles, 53(9–10), 619–633.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-7729-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Burnham, D. K., & Harris, M. B. (1992). Effects of real gender and labeled gender on adults' perceptions of infants. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153(2), 165–183.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1992.10753711.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Butler, J. (1993). Bodies that matter: On the discursive limits of “sex.” New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  19. Bushnell, E. W. (1985). The decline of visually guided reaching during infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 139–155.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(85)80002-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106(4), 676–713.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O'Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60(1), 70–76.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1131072.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Campbell, A., Shirley, L., Heywood, C., & Crook, C. (2000). Infants’ visual preference for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 479–498.  https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cherney, I. D., Kelly-Vance, L., Glover, K. G., Ruane, A., & Ryalls, B. O. (2003). The effects of stereotyped toys and gender on play assessment in children aged 18-47 months. Educational Psychology, 23, 95–105.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410303222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Civan, A., Teller, D. Y., & Palmer, J. (2005). Relations among spontaneous preferences, familiarized preferences and novelty effects: Measurements with forced-choice techniques. Infancy, 7, 111–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Cohen-Bendahan, C. C., van de Beek, C., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2005). Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: Methods and findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavior Review, 29, 353–384.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.004.
  26. Collaer, M. L., & Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioral sex differences: A role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 55–107.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.55.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Coleman, J. M., & Hong, Y. Y. (2008). Beyond nature and nurture: The influence of lay gender theories on self-stereotyping. Self and Identity, 7(1), 34–53.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860600980185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Columbo, J. (1997). Individual differences in infant cognition: methods, measures, and models. In J. Dobbing (Ed.), Developing brain and behavior: The role of lipids in infant formulas (pp. 339–385). London: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Crawley, S., & Broad, K. L. (2008). The construction of sex and sexualities. In J. Holstein & J. Gubrium (Eds.), Handbook of constructionist research (pp. 545–566). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  30. Dannemiller, J. L., & Stephens, B. R. (1988). A critical test of infant pattern preference models. Child Development, 59(1), 210–216.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130403.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2013). The nature-nurture debates: 25 years of challenges in understanding the psychology of gender. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 340–357.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613484767.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Endendijk, J. J., Groeneveld, M. G., van Berkel, S. R., Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., Mesman, J., & Bakersman-Kraneburg, M. J. (2013). Gender stereotypes in the family context: Mothers, fathers, and siblings. Sex Roles, 68, 577–590.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0265-4.
  33. Escudero, P., Robbins, R. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Sex-related preferences for real and doll faces versus real and toy objects in young infants and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 367–379.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.001.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Fagan, J. F. (1970). Memory in the infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9(2), 217–226.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Fagot, B. I. (1995). Psychosocial and cognitive determinants of early gender-role development. Annual Review of Sex Research, 6, 1–31.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10532528.1995.10559900.Google Scholar
  36. Fagot, B. I., & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parent reactions to sex-stereotyped behaviors: Age and sex effects. Child Development, 62, 617–628.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1131135.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Feinman, S., & Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at ten months: A second-order effect on infants' responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878–887.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Flom, R., & Johnson, S. (2010). The effects of adults’ affective expression and direction of visual gaze on 12-month-olds’ visual preferences following a 5 minute, 1-day or 1-month delay. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 64–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Francis, B. (2010). Gender, toys, and learning. Oxford Review of Education, 36, 325–344.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0305498100373227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gabbard, C., Caçola, P., & Rodrigues, L. P. (2008). A new inventory for assessing affordances in the home environment for motor development (AHEMD-SR). Early Childhood Education Journal, 36, 5–9.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-008-0235-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Goldberg, A. E., Kashy, D. A., & Smith, J. Z. (2012). Gender-typed play behavior in early childhood: Adopted children with lesbian, gay, and heterosexual parents. Sex Roles, 67(9–10), 503–515.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0198-3.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Gredebäck, G., Johnson, S., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Eye tracking in infancy research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(1), 1–19.  https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903325758.
  43. Green, V. A., Bigler, R., & Catherwood, D. (2004). The variability and flexibility of gender-typed toy play: A close look at children’s behavioral responses to counterstereotypic models. Sex Roles, 51, 371–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Haight, W. L., Parke, R. D., & Black, J. E. (1997). Mother’s and fathers’ beliefs about and spontaneous participation in their toddlers’ pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 271–290. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/23092492.
  45. Halim, M. L., & Ruble, D. (2010). Gender identity and stereotyping in early and middle childhood. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 495–525). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Halim, M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Zosuls, K. M., Lurve, L. E., & Greulich, F. K. (2014). Pink frilly dresses and the avoidance of all things “girly”: Children’s appearance rigidity and cognitive theories of gender development. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1091–1101.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034906.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones & Behavior, 54(3), 359–364.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hare-Mustin, R. (1978). A feminist approach to family therapy. Family Process, 17(2), 181–194.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1978.00181.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Hare-Mustin, R. (1987). The problem of gender in family therapy theory. Family Process, 26(1), 15–27.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1987.00015.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Hines, M., Pasterski, V., Spencer, D., Neufeld, S., Patalay, P., Hindmarsh, P. C., ... Acerini, C. L. (2016). Prenatal androgen exposure alters girls' responses to information indicating gender-appropriate behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1688), 20150125.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0125.
  52. Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Hunter, M. A., & Ames, E. W. (1988). A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. Advances in Infancy Research, 5, 69–95.Google Scholar
  54. Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender differences. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 16(1), 55–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Jacklin, C. N., DiPietro, J. A., & Maccoby, E. E. (1984). Sex-typing behavior and sex-typing pressure in child/parent interaction. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13, 413–425.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01541427.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Jadva, V., Hines, M., & Golombok, S. (2010). Infants’ preferences for toys, colors, and shapes: Sex differences and similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(6), 1261–1273.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-01-9618-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Jürgensen, M., Hiort, O., Holterhus, P.-M., & Thyen, U. (2007). Gender role behavior in children with XY karyotype and disorders of sex development. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 443–453.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Kane, E. W. (2006). “No way my boys are going to be like that!” Parents’ responses to children’s gender nonconformity. Gender & Society, 20(2), 149–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Karraker, K. H., Vogel, D. A., & Lake, M. A. (1995). Parents’ gender-stereotyped perceptions of newborn: The eye of the beholder revisited. Sex Roles, 33, 687–701.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01547725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Kleiner, K. A., & Banks, M. S. (1987). Stimulus energy does not account for 2-month-olds' face preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(4), 594–600.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Kohlberg, L. A. (1966). A cognitive–developmental analysis of children’s sex role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Lamminmåki, A., Hines, M., Kuiri-Hänninen, T., Kilpeläinen, L., Dunkel, L., & Sankilampi, U. (2012). Testosterone measured in infancy predicts subsequent sex-typed behavior in boys and in girls. Hormones and Behavior, 61(4), 611–616.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.013.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1980). Mothers, fathers and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children. Child Development, 51, 1237–1247.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Leaper, C. (2000). The social construction and socialization of gender during development. In P. H. Miller & E. K. Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist development psychology (pp. 127–152). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  65. Leinbach, M. D., & Fagot, B. I. (1993). Categorical habituation to male and female faces: Gender schematic processing in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(3), 317–332.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(93)80038-A.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. L. (1980). Gender-related schemata and constructive memory in children. Child Development, 51, 11–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Lieberson, S., & Bell, E. O. (1992). Children’s first names: An empirical study of social taste. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 511–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Lindsey, E. W., & Mize, J. (2000). Parent-child physical and pretense play: Links to children’s social competence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 565–591.Google Scholar
  69. Lindsey, E. W., & Mize, J. (2001). Contextual differences in parent-child play: Implications for children’s gender role development. Sex Roles, 44, 155–176.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010950919451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Lober, J. (1991). Dismantling Noah’s ark. In J. Lober & S. A. Farrell (Eds.), The social construction of gender (pp. 355–369). Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  71. Lober, J. (1994). Paradoxes of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University of Press.Google Scholar
  72. Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parent’s differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Mahoney, A. R., & Knudson-Martin, C. (2009). The social context of gendered power. In C. Knudson-Martin & A. R. Mahoney (Eds.), Couples, gender, and power: Creating change in intimate relationships (pp. 17–29). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  74. Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children's gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66(5), 1453–1471.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1983). The effects of sex-typing schemas on young children’s memory. Child Development, 54, 563–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903–933.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Martin, K. A. (2005). William wants a doll. Can he have one? Feminists, child care advisors, and gender-neutral child rearing. Gender & Society, 19(4), 456–479.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243204272968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. McDowell, T. (2015). Applying critical social theories to family therapy practice. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Baratz Dalke, K., Berenbaum, S. A., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Hines, M., & Schober, J. M. (2016). Gender assignment, reassignment and outcome in disorders of sex development: Update of the 2005 Consensus Conference. Hormone Research in Pædiatrics, 85(2), 112–118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Nelson, A. (2005). Children's toy collections in Sweden: A less gender-typed country? Sex Roles, 52(1–2), 93–102.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-1196-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Okruhlik, K. (1998). Gender and the biological sciences. In M. Curd & J. A. Cover (Eds.), Philosophy of science: The central issues (pp. 192–208). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  82. Oransky, M., & Marecek, J. (2009). “I'm not going to be a girl”: Masculinity and emotions in boys' friendships and peer groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24(2), 218–241.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558408329951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Pennell, G. E. (1994). Babes in Toyland: Learning an ideology of gender. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 359–364.Google Scholar
  84. Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22(5–6), 359–367.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0028839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31(9), 1109–1122.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p3331.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Reynolds, G. D. (2015). Research report: Infant visual attention and object recognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 285, 34–43.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.015.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  87. Rochat, P., & Goubet, N. (1995). Development of sitting and reaching in 5-to 6-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 18(1), 53–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Rubin, K. H., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: Parent’s views on sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44, 512–519.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1974.tb00905.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2002). Infants' responses to gender-inconsistent events. Infancy, 3, 531–542.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0304_07.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and 5-year-olds’ toy-choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 43–48.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00096.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Servin, A., Nordenström, A., Larsson, A., & Bohlin, G. (2003). Prenatal androgens and gender-typed behavior: A study of girls with mild and severe forms of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 440–450.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.440.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Shakin, M., Shakin, D., & Sternglanz, S. H. (1985). Infant clothing: Sex labeling for strangers. Sex Roles, 12, 955–964.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Slaby, R. G., & Frey, K. S. (1975). Development of gender constancy and selective attention to same-sex models. Child Development, 46, 849–856.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1128389.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Snow, M. E., Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Sex-of-child differences in father-child interaction at one year of age. Child Development, 54(1), 227–232.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Stennes, L. M., Burch, M. M., Sen, M. G., & Bauer, P. J. (2005). A longitudinal study of gendered vocabulary and communicative action in young children. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 75–88.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.75.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Teichner, G., Ames, E. W., & Kerig, P. K. (1997). The relation of infant crying and the sex of the infant to parents’ perceptions of the infant and themselves. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 34, 59–60.Google Scholar
  97. Teller, D. Y. (1979). The forced-choice preferential looking procedure: A psychophysical technique for use with human infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 135–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Todd, B. K., Barry, J. A., & Thommessen, S. A. (2016). Preferences for ‘gender-typed’ toys in boys and girls aged 9 to 32 months. Infant and Child Development. Advanced online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.
  99. Tenebaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38, 34–57.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.38.4.615.Google Scholar
  100. van de Beek, C., van Goozen, S. M., Buitelaar, J. K., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2009). Prenatal sex hormones (maternal and amniotic fluid) and gender-related play behavior in 13-month-old infants. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(1), 6–15.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9291-z.
  101. von Hofsten, C., & Fazel-Zandy, S. (1984). Development of visually guided hand orientation in reaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38(2), 208–219.Google Scholar
  102. Weinraub, M., Clemens, L. P., Sockloff, A., Ethridge, T., Gracely, E., & Myers, B. (1984). The development of sex role stereotypes in the third year: Relationships to gender labeling, gender identity, sex-types toy preference, and family characteristics. Child Development, 55(4), 1493–1503.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. M. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children’s toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 401–409.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.06.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Wetherford, M. J., & Cohen, L. B. (1973). Developmental changes in infant visual preferences for novelty and familiarity. Child Development, 44, 416–424.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Wilkinson, N., Paikan, A., Gredebäck, G., Rea, F., & Metta, G. (2014). Staring us in the face? An embodied theory of innate face preference. Developmental Science, 17(6), 809–825.  https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Wood, E., Demarais, S., & Gugula, S. (2002). The impact of parenting experience of gender stereotyped toy play of children. Sex Roles, 47, 39–49.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020679619728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Yelland, N., & Grieshaber, S. (1998). Blurring the edges. In N. Yelland (Ed.), Gender in early childhood (pp. 1–11). Florence, KY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge.Google Scholar
  108. Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for gender-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014053.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Human Development and Family ScienceUniversity of GeorgiaAthensUSA
  2. 2.Department of Human Development and Family ScienceNorth Dakota State UniversityFargoUSA

Personalised recommendations