Advertisement

Sex Roles

, Volume 73, Issue 1–2, pp 16–28 | Cite as

Pursuing Desires Rather Than Duties? The Motivational Content of Gender Stereotypes

  • Amanda M. Johnston
  • Amanda B. Diekman
Original Article

Abstract

In the current research we provide initial documentation of the motivational content of gender stereotypes. Drawing from regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1987, 1997), we distinguish between whether traits are perceived to be motivated by ideals, including hopes, dreams, and aspirations, and oughts, including responsibilities, duties, and obligations. In two studies of U.S. Midwestern undergraduates, participants rated the extent to which the traits of men or women are motivated by ideals or oughts. Study 1 included 137 introductory psychology students who rated gender stereotypic traits (e.g., competitiveness for men; sensitivity for women); Study 2 included 118 introductory psychology students who rated gender stereotypic or counterstereotypic traits (e.g., competitiveness for women; sensitivity for men). In both Studies 1 and 2, we demonstrate that people perceive women as especially motivated by ideals rather than oughts but men as equivalently motivated by ideals and oughts. These patterns emerge regardless of trait stereotypicality. In Study 3, we examined the relationship between perceptions of role flexibility and the perception of ideal motivation using a sample of 214 introductory psychology students from the Midwestern U.S. We found evidence that ascriptions of ideal motivation are associated with perceptions of role flexibility, which are greater for women. We discuss the implications of these beliefs for the legitimization of the existing gendered social system.

Keywords

Gender stereotypes Gender roles Motivation Social roles Stereotype content 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflicts of Interest

We have no conflicts of interest.

Informed Consent

All participants were required to sign an informed consent prior to participation.

References

  1. Allport, G. W. (1954/1979). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge: Perseus Books.Google Scholar
  2. Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 379–384. doi: 10.3758/BF03192707.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Banchefsky, S., Diekman, A., Johnston, A. (2013). Achieving equality vs. avoiding discrimination: Using regulatory framing to affect attitudes. Poster presented at the 2013 meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
  4. Bianchi, S. M. (2011). Family change and time allocation in American families. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 638, 21–44. doi: 10.1177/0002716211413731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bianchi, S. M., Milkie, M. A., Sayer, L. C., & Robinson, J. P. (2000). Is anyone doing the housework? Trends in the gender division of household labor. Social Forces, 79, 191–228. doi: 10.2307/2675569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bianchi, S. M., Milkie, M. A., Sayer, L. C., & Robinson, J. P. (2012). Housework: Who did, does or will do it, and how much does it matter? Social Forces, 91, 55–63. doi: 10.1093/sf/sos120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brescoll, V., & LaFrance, M. (2004). The correlates and consequences of newspaper reports of research on sex differences. Psychological Science, 15, 515–520. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00712.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brescoll, V. L., Uhlman, E. L., & Newman, G. E. (2013). The effects of system-justifying motives on endorsement of essentialist explanations for gender differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 891–908. doi: 10.1037/a0034701.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Broverman, I. K., Vogel, S. R., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F., & Rosenkrantz, P. S. (1972). Sex-role stereotypes: A current appraisal. Journal of Social Issues, 28, 59–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1972.tb00018.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Center for American Women and Politics. (2015). Women in elective office 2015: Fact Sheet. New Brunswick: Rutgers. Retrieved from http://www.cawp.rutgers.edu/fast_facts/levels_of_office/documents/elective.pdf.
  11. Croft, A., Schmader, T., & Block, K. (2015). An underexamined inequality: Cultural and psychological barriers to men’s engagements with communal roles. Personality and Social Psychology Review. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/1088868314564789.
  12. Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J. (2006). Exposure to scientific theories affects women’s math performance. Science, 314, 435. doi: 10.1126/science.1131100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991–1004. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. doi: 10.1177/0146167200262001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Diekman, A. B., & Goodfriend, W. (2006). Rolling with the changes: A role congruity perspective on gender norms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 369–383. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00312.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Diekman, A. B., Eagly, A. H., & Kulesa, P. (2002). Accuracy and bias in stereotypes about the social and political attitudes of women and men. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 268–282. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Diekman, A. B., Goodfriend, W., & Goodwin, S. (2004). Dynamic stereotypes of power: Perceived change and stability in gender hierarchies. Sex Roles, 50, 201–215. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000015552.22775.44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Diekman, A. B., Eagly, A. H., Mladinic, A., & Ferreira, M. C. (2005). Dynamic stereotypes about women and men in Latin America and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 209–226. doi: 10.1177/0022022104272902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Diekman, A. B., Eagly, A. H., & Johnston, A. M. (2010). Social structure. In J. F. Dovidio, M. Hewstone, P. Glick, & V. M. Esses (Eds.), The Sage handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 209–224). New York: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2003). The malleability of sex differences in response to changing social roles. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology (pp. 103–115). Washington, D.C.: APA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Eagly, A. H., & Kite, M. E. (1987). Are stereotypes of nationalities applied to both women and men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 451–462. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1989). Gender stereotypes and attitudes toward women and men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 543–558. doi: 10.1177/0146167289154008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735–754. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.2.109.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Goni-Legaz, S., Ollo-Lopez, A., & Bayo-Moriones, A. (2010). The division of household labor in Spanish dual earner couples: Testing three theories. Sex Roles, 53, 515–529. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9840-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  30. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319.
  31. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1–46. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60381-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Johnston, A. M., & Diekman, A. B. (2013, January). Perceiving desires, not duties: Believing women are ideally motivated legitimizes the existing system. In M. Williams (Chair), When and why women step back from status: The enduring and self-reinforcing power of traditional gender roles. Symposium conducted at the 2013 meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
  34. Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences for specific and diffuse forms of system justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 498–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.498.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jost, J. T., Kivetz, Y., Rubini, M., Guermandi, G., & Mosso, C. (2005). System-justifying functions of complementary regional and ethnic stereotypes: Cross-national evidence. Social Justice Research, 18, 305–333. doi: 10.1007/s11211-005-6827-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kay, A. C., & Jost, J. T. (2003). Complementary justice: Effects of “poor but happy” and “poor but honest” stereotype exemplars on system justification and implicit activation of the justice motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 823–837. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lenton, A. P., Sedikides, C., & Bruder, M. (2009). A latent semantic analysis of gender stereotype-consistency and narrowness in American English. Sex Roles, 60, 269–278. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9534-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Martin, C. L., & Parker, S. (1995). Folk theories about sex and race differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 45–57. doi: 10.1177/0146167295211006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). When men break the gender rules: Status incongruity and backlash against modest men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 11, 140–151. doi: 10.1037/a0018093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 173–176. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1996). Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social norms by unwitting actors. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 28, pp. 161–209). San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
  44. Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004–1010. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1004.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic ingroup bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 494–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 165–179. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sayer, L. C., Bianchi, S. M., & Robinson, J. P. (2004). Are parents investing less in children? Trends in mothers’ and fathers’ time with children. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 1–43. doi: 10.1086/386270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sczesny, S., Bosak, J., Neff, D., & Schyns, B. (2004). Gender stereotypes and the attribution of leadership traits: A cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 51, 631–645. doi: 10.1007/s11199-004-0715-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Twenge, J. M. (1997). Changes in masculine and feminine traits over time: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 36, 305–325. doi: 10.1007/BF02766650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. U.S. Department of Labor. (2014). Women in the labor force: A databook (2014 ed., Report No. 1052). Washington, DC: U. S. Bureau of Labor Statistics.Google Scholar
  52. Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes: A multination study. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Houston-Clear LakeHoustonUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyMiami UniversityOxfordUSA

Personalised recommendations