Sex Roles

, Volume 58, Issue 9–10, pp 702–712 | Cite as

Variations of Gender–math Stereotype Content Affect Women’s Vulnerability to Stereotype Threat

  • Dustin B. Thoman
  • Paul H. White
  • Niwako Yamawaki
  • Hirofumi Koishi
Original Article

Abstract

To determine whether variations in stereotype content salience moderates stereotype threat effects, 66 US female undergraduate students were given a standardized math exam, and the salience of specific gender–math stereotype content was manipulated before the exam. Women exerted more effort on each problem and performed better on a math exam when threatened with an effort-based stereotype compared to when threatened with the ability-based stereotype or control (where no stereotype was explicitly mentioned). Implications of these results are discussed in terms of stereotype and social identity threat theory, as well as how the socio-cultural salience of ability versus other components of the gender–math stereotype may impact women who pursue math and science-based domains.

Keywords

Gender stereotypes Stereotype threat Sex Mathematical ability Academic motivation 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Portions of this research were funded by a National Science Foundation Fellowship for the first author to participate in the East Asia Summer Institute Program. We also thank Carol Sansone for her helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript, as well as two very helpful anonymous reviewers for their comments.

References

  1. Aronson, J., Fried, C., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When White men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students. Child Development, 66, 1215–1234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 504–533, 4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  5. Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the effects of stereotypes threat on women’s leadership aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 276–287.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Deaux, K., & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 504–533, 4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  7. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women’s educational and occupational choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Educational Testing Service (1995). Practicing to take the general test: Big book. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
  11. Fazio, R. H., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1977). Dissonance and self perception: An integrated view of each theory’s proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 464–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 645–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Good, C., Dweck, C. S., & Rattan, A. (2005). Perceiving a malleable-ability versus a fixed-ability environment: The effect of women’s sense of belonging to math. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. New Orleans, LA, January.Google Scholar
  14. Hess, R. D., & Azuma, H. (1991). Cultural support for schooling: Contrasts between Japan and the United States. Educational Researcher, 20, 2–8.Google Scholar
  15. Inzlicht, M., & Good, C. (2006). How environments can threaten academic performance, self-knowledge, and sense of belonging. In S. Levin & C. van Laar (Eds.), Stigma and group inequality: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 129–150). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Kurtz-Costez, B., McCall, R. J., Kinlaw, C. R., Wiesen, C. A., & Joyner, H. (2005). What does it mean to be smart? The development about children’s beliefs about intelligence in Germany and the United States. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 217–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Murphy, M., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18, 879–885.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. National Science Foundation (1996). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 1996 (National Science Foundation Publication No. 96–311). Arlington, VA: Author.Google Scholar
  19. Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the missing motivator in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10, 175–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Seymour, E., & Hewitt, N. M. (1997). Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave the sciences. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  21. Sloan, L. R., Wilburn, G., Fenton, T., Craig-Henderson, K., & Starr, B. J. (2004). Out-group presence, but not overt negative stereotype reminders, produce stereotype threat performance decrements in in-group settings. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL, April.Google Scholar
  22. Smith, J. L. (2004). Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of meditational variables and new performance goal directions. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 177–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2001). Development of the domain identification measure: A tool for investigating stereotype threat effects. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61, 1040–1057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, 47, 179–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype threat and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Swim, J. K., & Stangor, C. (1998). Prejudice: The target’s perspective. San Diego, CA: Academic.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dustin B. Thoman
    • 1
  • Paul H. White
    • 1
  • Niwako Yamawaki
    • 2
  • Hirofumi Koishi
    • 3
  1. 1.University of UtahSalt Lake CityUSA
  2. 2.Brigham Young UniversityProvoUSA
  3. 3.Kobe UniversityKobeJapan

Personalised recommendations