Science & Education

, Volume 26, Issue 1–2, pp 93–105 | Cite as

Explicitly Teaching Critical Thinking Skills in a History Course

Article

Abstract

Critical thinking skills are often assessed via student beliefs in non-scientific ways of thinking, (e.g, pseudoscience). Courses aimed at reducing such beliefs have been studied in the STEM fields with the most successful focusing on skeptical thinking. However, critical thinking is not unique to the sciences; it is crucial in the humanities and to historical thinking and analysis. We investigated the effects of a history course on epistemically unwarranted beliefs in two class sections. Beliefs were measured pre- and post-semester. Beliefs declined for history students compared to a control class and the effect was strongest for the honors section. This study provides evidence that a humanities education engenders critical thinking. Further, there may be individual differences in ability or preparedness in developing such skills, suggesting different foci for critical thinking coursework.

References

  1. Aarnio, K., & Lindeman, M. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, education, and thinking styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1227–1236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abrami, P. C., et al. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills and dispositions: a stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 1102–1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Allchin, D. (2004). Pseudohistory and pseudoscience. Science & Education, 13, 179–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Arum, R., & Roksa, J. (2011). Academically adrift: limited learning on college campuses. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  5. Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn?: a taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boudry, M. (2013). Loki’s wager and Laudan’s error: on genuine and territorial demarcation. In M. Pigliucci & M. Boudry (Eds.), Philosophy of pseudoscience: reconsidering the demarcation problem (pp. 79–100). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bridgstock, M. (2003). Paranormal beliefs among science students. Australasian Science, 24(4), 33–35.Google Scholar
  8. Bunge, M. (2010). Knowledge: genuine and bogus. Science & Education, 20(5–6), 411–438.Google Scholar
  9. Davies, M. (2013). Critical thinking and the disciplines reconsidered. Higher Education Research & Development, 32(4), 529–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. DeRobertis, M. M., & Delaney, P. A. (1993). A survey of the attitudes of university students to astrology and astronomy. Journal of the Royal Astronomical Society of Canada, 87(1), 34–50.Google Scholar
  11. Diamond, J. (2005). Collapse: how societies choose to fail or succeed. New York: Penguin Group.Google Scholar
  12. Dougherty, M. J. (2004). Educating believers: research demonstrates that courses in skepticism can effectively decrease belief in the paranormal. Skeptic, 10(4), 31–35.Google Scholar
  13. Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: a statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction. Millbrae: The California Academic Press.Google Scholar
  14. Feder, K. (1984). Irrationality and archaeology. American Antiquity, 49(3), 525–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Feder, K. (1995). Ten years after: surveying misconceptions about the human past. Cultural Resource Management, 18(3), 10–14.Google Scholar
  16. Feder, K. (2010). Frauds, myths, and mysteries: science and pseudoscience in archaeology (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  17. Fitzgerald, J., & Baird, V. A. (2011). Taking a step back: teaching critical thinking by distinguishing appropriate type of evidence. Political Science and Politics, 44(3), 619–624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Franz, T. M., & Green, K. H. (2013). The impact of an interdisciplinary learning community course on pseudoscientific reasoning in first-year science students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 13(5), 90–105.Google Scholar
  19. Freidel, D. (2007). Betraying the Maya. Archaeology Magazine, 60(2), 36–41.Google Scholar
  20. Goode, E. (2002). Education, scientific knowledge, and belief in the paranormal. Skeptical Inquirer, 26(1), 24–27.Google Scholar
  21. Gray, T. (1985). Changing unsubstantiated belief: testing the ignorance hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 17, 263–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Harrold, F. B., & Eve, R. A. (Eds.). (1987). Cult archaeology and creationism: understanding pseudoscientific beliefs about the past. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press.Google Scholar
  23. Holtorf, C. (2005). From Stonehenge to Las Vegas: archaeology as popular culture. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  24. Johnson, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2004). Is knowledge of science associated with higher skepticism of pseudoscientific claims? The American Biology Teacher, 66(8), 536–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kane, M. J., Core, T. J., & Hunt, R. R. (2010). Bias versus bias: harnessing hindsight to reveal paranormal belief change beyond demand characteristics. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17(2), 206–212. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.2.206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Karimi, F., & Sutton, J. (2014). Maryland mom kills two of her children during attempted exorcism. CNN. http://www.cnn.com/2014/01/19/justice/maryland-exorcism-deaths/. Accessed 20 Feb.
  27. Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Losh, S. C., & Nzekwe, B. (2011). Creatures in the classroom: preservice teacher beliefs about fantastic beasts, magic, extraterrestrials, evolution and creationism. Science & Education, 20, 473–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McAnany, P. A., & Negrón, T. G. (2009). Bellicose rulers and climatological peril? Retrofitting 21st century woes on 8th century Maya society. In In questioning collapse: human resilience, ecological vulnerability, and the aftermath of empire. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. McAnany, P. A., & Yoffee, N. (Eds.) (2010). Questioning collapse: human resilience, ecological vulnerability, and the aftermath of empire. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Niu, L., Behar-Horenstein, L. S., & Garvan, C. W. (2013). Do instructional interventions influence college students’ critical thinking skills? A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 9, 114–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. NSF. (2014). Chapter 7: Science and technology: public attitudes and understanding. In Science and Engineering Indicators 2014. National Science Foundation, 7–1–7-37.Google Scholar
  33. Pascarella, E. T., Blaich, C., Martin, G. L., & Hanson, J. M. (2011). How robust are the findings of academically adrift? Change: The Magazine of Higher Learning, 43(3), 20–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Paul, R. (1995). Critical thinking: how to prepare students for a rapidly changing world. Rohnert Park: Foundation for Critical Thinking.Google Scholar
  35. Pew (2013). Public’s knowledge of science and technology. Pew Research Center, Washington, D.C. (April). http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/04-22-13%20Science%20knowledge%20Release.pdf. Accessed 23 Nov 2014.
  36. Pyburn, A. (2006). The politics of collapse. Archaeologies, 2(1), 3–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ren, A. C. (2006). Maya archaeology and the political and cultural identity of contemporary Maya in Guatemala. Archaeologies, 2(1), 8–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rice, T. W. (2003). Believe it or not: religious and other paranormal beliefs in the United States. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, 95–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ryan, T. J., Brown, J., Johnson, A., Sanburg, C., & Schildmeier, M. (2004). Science literacy and belief in the paranormal—an empirical test. Skeptic, 10(4), 12–13.Google Scholar
  40. Sagan, C. (1996). The demon-haunted world: science as a candle in the dark. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
  41. Smith, M. U., & Siegel, H. (2004). Knowing, believing, and understanding: what goals for science education? Science & Education, 13, 553–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tobacyk, J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 23, 94–98.Google Scholar
  43. Turgut, H. (2011). The context of demarcation in nature of science teaching: the case of astrology. Science & Education, 20(5–6), 491–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wesp, R., & Montgomery, K. (1998). Developing critical thinking through the study of paranormal phenomena. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 275–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive: beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 767–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anne Collins McLaughlin
    • 1
  • Alicia Ebbitt McGill
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyNorth Carolina State UniversityRaleighUSA
  2. 2.Department of HistoryNorth Carolina State UniversityRaleighUSA

Personalised recommendations