Science & Education

, Volume 23, Issue 5, pp 1051–1074 | Cite as

Darwin’s Difficulties and Students’ Struggles with Trait Loss: Cognitive-Historical Parallelisms in Evolutionary Explanation

Article

Abstract

Although historical changes in scientific ideas sometimes display striking similarities with students’ conceptual progressions, some scholars have cautioned that such similarities lack meaningful commonalities. In the history of evolution, while Darwin and his contemporaries often used natural selection to explain evolutionary trait gain or increase, they struggled to use it to convincingly account for cases of trait loss or decrease. This study examines Darwin’s evolutionary writings about trait gain and loss in the Origin of Species (On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. D. Appleton, New York, 1859) and compares them to written evolutionary explanations for trait gain and loss in a large (n > 500), cross-cultural and cross-sectional sample (novices and experts from the USA and Korea). Findings indicate that significantly more students and experts applied natural selection to cases of trait gain, but like Darwin and his contemporaries, they more often applied ‘use and disuse’ and ‘inheritance of acquired characteristics’ to episodes of trait loss. Although the parallelism between Darwin’s difficulties and students’ struggles with trait loss are striking, significant differences also characterize explanatory model structure. Overall, however, students and scientists struggles to explain trait loss—which is a very common phenomenon in the history of life—appear to transcend time, place, and level of biological expertise. The significance of these findings for evolution education are discussed; in particular, the situated nature of biological reasoning, and the important role that the history of science can play in understanding cognitive constraints on science learning.

References

  1. Alters, B. J., & Nelson, C. E. (2002). Perspective: Teaching evolution in higher education. Evolution, 56(10), 1891–1901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beggrow, E. P., & Nehm, R. H. (2012). Students’ mental models of evolutionary causation: Natural selection and genetic drift. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 5(3), 429–444.Google Scholar
  3. Berti, A. E., Toneatti, L., & Rosati, V. (2010). Children’s conceptions about the origin of species: A study of Italian children’s conceptions with and without instruction. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19(4), 506–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bishop, B. A., & Anderson, C. W. (1990). Student conceptions of natural selection and its role in evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27(5), 415–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bizzo, N. M. V. (1994). From down house landlord to Brazilian high school students: What has happened to evolutionary knowledge on the way? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(5), 537–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bocking, S. (1988). Alpheus Spring Packard and cave fauna in the evolution debate. Journal of the History of Biology, 21(3), 425–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowler, P. J. (2009). Evolution: The history of an idea. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  8. Brooks, D. R., & McLennan, D. A. (1993). Macroevolutionary patterns of morphological diversification among parasitic flatworms (Platyhelminthes: Cercomeria). Evolution, 47(2), 495–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Campbell, D. (1911). Plant life and evolution, American nature series. New York: Henry Holt & Co.Google Scholar
  10. Caruso, C., Rigato, E., & Minelli, A. (2012). Finalism and adaptationism in contemporary biological literature. Atti dell’Istituto Veneto di Scienze, Lettere ed Arti. Classe di Scienze Fisiche, Matematiche e Naturali, 170(1–2-3), 69–76.Google Scholar
  11. Clement, J. (1983). A conceptual model discussed by Galileo and used intuitively by physics students. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 325–339). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Corsi, P. (1988). The age of Lamarck: Evolutionary theories in France, 1790–1830. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  13. Corsi, P. (2005). Before Darwin: Transformist concepts in European natural history. Journal of the History of Biology, 38, 67–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Corsi, P. (2011). Jean-Baptiste Lamarck. From myth to history. In E. Jablonka & S. Gissis (Eds.), Transformations of Lamarckism: From subtle fluids to molecular biology (pp. 12–28). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  15. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. New York: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
  16. Driver, R. (1983). The pupil as scientist? Milton Keynes: The Open University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Endler, J. A. (1986). Natural selection in the wild. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Espinasa, M., & Espinasa, L. (2008). Losing sight of regressive evolution. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 1(4), 509–516.Google Scholar
  19. Evans, E. M., Spiegel, A. N., Gram, W., Frazier, B. N., Tare, M., & Thompson, S. (2010). A conceptual guide to natural history museum visitors’ understanding of evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47, 326–353.Google Scholar
  20. Futuyma, D. J. (2013). Evolution (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  21. Garman, H. (1892). A synopsis of the reptiles and amphibians of Illinois. Illinois State Laboratory of Natural History, 3(8), 215–385.Google Scholar
  22. Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gelman, S. A., & Rhodes, M. (2012). “Two-thousand years of stasis”: How psychological essentialism impedes evolutionary understanding. In K. S. Rosengren, S. Brem, E. M. Evans, & G. Sinatra (Eds.), Evolution challenges: Integrating research and practice in teaching and learning about evolution (pp. 3–21). Cambridge: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Ghiselin, M. T. (1994). Darwin’s language may seem teleological, but his thinking is another matter. Biology and Philosophy, 9(4), 489–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Greiffenhagen, C., & Sherman, W. (2008). Kuhn and conceptual change: On the analogy between conceptual changes in science and children. Science & Education, 17(1), 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Held, L. I. (2009). Quirks of human anatomy: An evo-devo look at the human body. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Jensen, M. S., & Finley, F. N. (1996). Changes in students’ understanding of evolution resulting from different curricular and instructional strategies. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(8), 879–900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kampourakis, K., & Nehm, R. H. (in press). History and philosophy of science and the teaching of evolution: students’ conceptions and explanations. In M. R. Matthews (Ed.), International Handbook of Research in History, Philosophy and Science Teaching. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
  30. Kampourakis, K., & Zogza, V. (2007). Students’ preconceptions about evolution: How accurate is the characterization as “Lamarckian” when considering the history of evolutionary thought? Science & Education, 16(3–5), 393–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kampourakis, K., & Zogza, V. (2008). Students’ intuitive explanations of the causes of homologies and adaptations. Science & Education, 17(1), 27–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kelemen, D. (2012). Teleological minds: How natural intuitions about agency and purpose influence learning about evolution. In K. S. Rosengren, S. K. Brem, E. M. Evans, & G. M. Sinatra (Eds.), Evolution challenges: Integrating research and practice in teaching and learning about evolution (pp. 66–92). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kim, S. Y., & Nehm, R. H. (2011). A crosscultural comparison of Korean and American science teachers’ views of evolution and the nature of science. International Journal of Science Education, 33(2), 197–227.Google Scholar
  34. Lee, M. S., & Lee, K. J. (2006). Analysis of student conceptions in evolution based on science history. Journal of Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(1), 25–39.Google Scholar
  35. Lennox, J. G. (1993). Darwin was a teleologist. Biology and Philosophy, 8(4), 409–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lennox, J. G. (1994). Teleology by another name: A reply to Ghiselin. Biology and Philosophy, 9(4), 493–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1(1), 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument: Charles Darwin and the genesis of modern evolutionary thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  39. National Research Council (NRC). (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core Ideas. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.Google Scholar
  40. Nehm, R. H., Beggrow, E. P., Opfer, J. E., & Ha, M. (2012). Reasoning about natural selection: Diagnosing contextual competency using the ACORNS instrument. The American Biology Teacher, 74(2), 92–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nehm, R. H., & Ha, M. (2011). Item feature effects in evolution assessment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(3), 237–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Nehm, R. H., Ha, M., Rector, M., Opfer, J. E., Perrin, L., Ridgway, J., et al. (2010). Scoring guide for the open response instrument (ORI) and evolutionary gain and loss test (ACORNS). Technical Report of National Science Foundation REESE Project 0909999.Google Scholar
  43. Nehm, R. H., & Reilly, L. (2007). Biology majors’ knowledge and misconceptions of natural selection. BioScience, 57(3), 263–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nehm, R. H., & Schonfeld, I. S. (2007). Does increasing biology teacher knowledge of evolution and the nature of science lead to greater preference for the teaching of evolution in schools? Journal of Science Teacher Education, 18(5), 699–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nehm, R. H., & Schonfeld, I. S. (2010). The future of natural selection knowledge measurement: A reply to Anderson et al. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47(3), 358–362.Google Scholar
  46. Nersessian, N. (1989). Conceptual change in science and in science education. Synthese, 80, 163–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Opfer, J. E., Nehm, R. H., & Ha, M. (2012). Cognitive foundations for science assessment design: Knowing what students know about evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 49(6), 744–777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Packard, A. S. (1893). Zoology for high schools and colleges (8th ed.). American Science Series—Advanced Course. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
  49. Passmore, C., & Stewart, J. (2002). A modeling approach to teaching evolutionary biology in high schools. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(3), 185–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Passmore, C., Stewart, J., & Zoellner, B. (2005). Providing high school students with opportunities to reason like evolutionary biologists. The American Biology Teacher, 67(4), 214–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pigliucci, M., & Müller, G. (Eds.). (2010). Evolution: The extended synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  52. Robbins, P., & Aydede, M. (2009). A short primer on situated cognition. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 1–10). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Samarapungavan, A., & Wiers, R. W. (1997). Children’s thoughts on the origin of species: A study of explanatory coherence. Cognitive Science, 21(2), 147–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Settlage, J., Jr. (1994). Conceptions of natural selection: A snapshot of the sense‐making process. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(5), 449–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Van Driel, J. H., De Vos, W., & Verloop, N. (1998). Relating students’ reasoning to the history of science: The case of chemical equilibrium. Research in Science Education, 28(2), 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1987). Theories of knowledge restructuring in development. Review of Educational Research, 51, 51–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wandersee, J. H. (1985). Can the history of science help science educators anticipate student’s misconceptions. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 23, 581–597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wiser, M., & Carey, S. (1983). When heat and temperature were one. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 267–297). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Teaching and LearningThe Ohio State UniversityColumbusUSA
  2. 2.Center for Science and Mathematics Education, Ecology and EvolutionStony Brook UniversityStony BrookUSA

Personalised recommendations