Advertisement

Science & Education

, Volume 22, Issue 3, pp 677–694 | Cite as

Healing, Mental Energy in the Physics Classroom: Energy Conceptions and Trust in Complementary and Alternative Medicine in Grade 10–12 Students

  • Annika M. Svedholm
  • Marjaana Lindeman
Article

Abstract

Lay conceptions of energy often conflict with scientific knowledge, hinder science learning and scientific literacy, and provide a basis for ungrounded beliefs. In a sample of Finnish upper secondary school students, energy was attributed with features of living and animate beings and thought of as a mental property. These ontologically confused conceptions (OCC) were associated with trust in complementary and alternative medicine (CAM), and independent of scientifically valid conceptions. Substance-based energy conceptions followed the correlational pattern of OCC, rather than scientific conceptions. OCC and CAM decreased both during the regular school physics curriculum and after a lesson targeted at the ontological confusions. OCC and CAM were slightly less common among students with high actively open-minded thinking, low trust in intuition and high need for cognition. The findings are discussed in relation to the goals of scientific education.

Keywords

Cognitive Style Secondary School Student Mental Property Ontological Category Energy Conception 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

The research was supported by grants from Nylands Nation and the Finnish Cultural Foundation to the first author. We thank all the teachers whose courses the study was carried out in for their time and collaboration.

References

  1. Amin, T. G. (2009). Conceptual metaphor meets conceptual change. Human Development, 52, 165–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Astin, J. (1998). Why patients use alternative medicine: results of a national study. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 1548–1553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bertrams, A., & Dickhäuser, O. (2009). High-school students’ need for cognition, self-control capacity, and school achievement: Testing a mediation hypothesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 135–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bishop, F. L., Yardley, L., & Lewith, G. T. (2007). A systematic review of beliefs involved in the use of complementary and alternative medicine. Journal of Health Psychology, 12, 851–867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brewe, E. (2011). Energy as a substancelike quantity that flows: Theoretical considerations and pedagogical consequences. Physical Review Special Topics—Physics Education Research, 7, 1–14.Google Scholar
  6. Brookes, D. T., & Etkina, E. (2007). Using conceptual metaphor and functional grammar to explore how language used in physics affects student learning. Physical Review Special Topics—Physics Education Research, 3, 1–16.Google Scholar
  7. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carey, S. (2000). Science education as conceptual change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 13–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & De Leeuw, N. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 27–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Deniz, H., Donnelly, L. A., & Yilmaz, I. (2008). Exploring the factors related to acceptance of evolutionary theory among Turkish preservice biology teachers: Toward a more informative conceptual ecology for biological evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45, 420–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. diSessa, A. A., Gillespie, N. M., & Esterly, J. B. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28, 843–900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Driver, R., Squires, A., Rushworth, P., & Wood-Robinson, V. (1994). Making sense of secondary science. Research into children’s ideas. London, New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  14. Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Furnham, A. (2007). Are modern health worries, personality and attitudes to science associated with the use of complementary and alternative medicine? British Journal of Health Psychology, 12, 229–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Grzywacz, J. G., Lang, W., Suerken, C., Quandt, S. A., Bell, R. A., & Arcury, T. A. (2005). Age, race, and ethnicity in the use of complementary and alternative medicine for health self-management: evidence from the 2002 National Health Interview Survey. Journal of Aging and Health, 17, 547–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gupta, A., Hammer, D., & Redish, E. F. (2010). The case for dynamic models of learners’ ontologies in physics. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19, 285–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hammer, D., & Elby, A. (2003). Tapping epistemological resources for learning physics. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12, 53–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hammer, D., Gupta, A., & Redish, E. F. (2011). On static and dynamic intuitive ontologies. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20, 163–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hodgkinson, G. P., Langan-Fox, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2008). Intuition: A fundamental bridging construct in the behavioural sciences. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jeppsson, F., Haglund, J., Amin, T. G., & Strömdahl, H. (2012). Exploring the use of conceptual metaphors in solving problems on entropy. Journal of the Learning Sciences. doi: 10.1080/10508406.2012.691926.
  24. Johnson, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2004). Is knowledge of science associated with higher skepticism of pseudoscientific claims? The American Biology Teacher, 66, 536–548.Google Scholar
  25. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kane, M. J., Core, T. J., & Hunt, R. R. (2010). Bias versus bias: Harnessing hindsight to reveal paranormal belief change beyond demand characteristics. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 206–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development. Psychological Review, 88, 197–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Keil, F. C. (1983). On the emergence of semantic and conceptual distinctions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 357–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lindeman, M. (2011). Biases in intuitive reasoning and belief in complementary and alternative medicine. Psychology & Health, 26, 371–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2006). Paranormal beliefs: Their dimensionality and correlates. European Journal of Personality, 20, 585–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lindeman, M., & Saher, M. (2007). Vitalism, purpose and superstition. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 33–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., & Green, B. (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: Naive beliefs about the motion of objects. Science, 210, 1139–1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Nordine, J., Krajcik, J., & Fortus, D. (2011). Transforming energy instruction in middle school to support integrated understanding and future learning. Science Education, 95, 670–699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. OECD-PISA. (1999). Measuring student knowledge and skills: A new framework for assessment. Paris: OECD.Google Scholar
  36. Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naive physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rössler, W., Lauber, C., Angst, J., Haker, H., Gamma, A., Eich, D., et al. (2006). The use of complementary and alternative medicine in the general population: results from a longitudinal community study. Psychological Medicine, 37, 73–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ryan, T. J., Brown, J., Johnson, A., Sanberg, C., & Schildmier, M. (2004). Science literacy and belief in the paranormal: An empirical test. Skeptic, 10, 12–13.Google Scholar
  39. Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
  40. Sá, W., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sadowski, C. J., & Gülgöz, S. (1996). Elaborative processing mediates the relationship between need for cognition and academic performance. The Journal of Psychology, 130, 303–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Saglam-Arslan, A. (2010). Cross-grade comparison of students’ understanding of energy concepts. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19, 303–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sinatra, G. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.). (2003). Intentional conceptual change. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  45. Slotta, J. D. (2011). In defense of Chi’s ontological incompatibility hypothesis. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20, 151–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Slotta, J. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology training. Cognition and Instruction, 24, 261–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Joram, E. (1995). Assessing students’ misclassifications of physics concepts: An ontological basis for conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 373–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Smith, M. U. (2010). Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: II. Pedagogical issues. Science & Education, 19, 539–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith, M. U., & Siegel, H. (2004). Knowing, believing, and understanding: What goals for science education? Science & Education, 13, 553–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 342–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Svedholm, A. M., & Lindeman, M. (2012). The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. British Journal of Psychology. Available online ahead of print, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2012.02118.x.
  52. Svedholm, A. M., Lindeman, M., & Lipsanen, J. (2010). Believing in the purpose of events: Why does it occur, and is it supernatural? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 252–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tindle, H. A., Davis, R. B., Phillips, R. S., & Eisenberg, D. M. (2005). Trends in use of complementary and alternative medicine by US adults: 1997–2002. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 11, 42–49.Google Scholar
  54. Trumper, R. (1993). Children’s energy concepts: A cross-age study. International Journal of Science Education, 15, 139–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wheeler, P., & Hyland, M. E. (2008). Dispositional predictors of complementary medicine and vitamin use in students. Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 516–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wiser, M., & Amin, T. (2001). “Is heat hot?” Inducing conceptual change by integrating everyday and scientific perspectives on thermal phenomena. Learning and Instruction, 11, 331–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Division of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Institute of Behavioural SciencesUniversity of HelsinkiUniversity of HelsinkiFinland

Personalised recommendations