Advertisement

Research in Science Education

, 36:269 | Cite as

The Effect of Verbal and Visuo-Spatial Abilities on the Development of Knowledge of the Earth

  • Eve Kikas
Article

Abstract

Difficulties in students’ understanding of the spherical model of the Earth have been shown in previous studies. One of the reasons for these difficulties lies in beliefs and preliminary knowledge that hinder the interpretation of the scientific knowledge, the other reason may lie in the low level of verbal and visuo-spatial abilities. The study aims to investigate the effect of verbal and visuo-spatial abilities, but also that of preliminary knowledge on the later development of the knowledge of the Earth in school. 176 schoolchildren (96 boys and 80 girls) from five schools were tested; the mean age of the children during the first interview was seven years and eight months. All students were interviewed twice – in grades 1 and 2, before and after they had learnt the topic in school. Factual, scientific and synthetic knowledge was assessed. The facilitative effect of visuo-spatial and verbal abilities and preliminary factual and scientific knowledge on students’ knowledge of astronomy after having learnt the topic in school was shown. In contrast, the hindering effect of synthetic knowledge was not found.

Key Words

conceptual development elementary astronomy factual knowledge learning scientific knowledge synthetic knowledge 

References

  1. Albanese, A., & Vicentini, M. (1997). Why do we believe that an atom is colourless? Reflections about the teaching of the particle model. Science and Education, 6, 251–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baxter, J. (1995). Children’s understanding of astronomy and the Earth sciences. In S. Glynn, & R. Duit (Eds.), Learning science in the schools (pp. 155–178). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1994). The development of children’s ability to use spatial representations. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development, 25 (pp. 157–199). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Carretero, M., & Voss, J. (Eds.) (1994). Cognitive and instructional processes in history and the social sciences. New Jersey and Hove: LEA.Google Scholar
  5. Chi, M., & Rees, E. (1983). A learning framework for development. Contributions to Human Development, 9, 71–107.Google Scholar
  6. Diakidoy, I.-A., & Kendeou, P. (2001). Facilitating conceptual change in astronomy: A comparison of the effectiveness of two instructional approaches. Learning and Instruction, 11, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Diakidoy, I.-A., Vosniadou, S., & Hawks, J. (1997). Conceptual change in astronomy: Models of the Earth and of the day/night cycle in American-Indian children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, 159–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hoyle, R. (Ed.) (1995). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  9. Kikas, E. (2003). Constructing knowledge beyond senses: Worlds too big and small to see. In A. Toomela (Ed.), Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind (pp. 211–227). Westport, Connecticut and London: Ablex.Google Scholar
  10. Kurdek, L., & Sinclair, R. (2001). Predicting reading and mathematics achievement in fourth-grade children from kindergarten readiness scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 451–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Luria, A. R. (1969). Vyshije korkovyje funktsii tsheloveka i ikh narushenija pri lokal’nykh porazenijakh mozga. (Higher cortical functions in man and their disturbances in local brain lesions.). Moscow: Izdatel’stvo Moskovskogo Universiteta.Google Scholar
  12. Mali, G., & Howe, A. (1979). Development of Earth and gravity concepts among Nepali children. Science Education, 63, 685–691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Maria, K. (1996). A case study of conceptual change in a young child. The Elementary School Journal, 98, 69–89.Google Scholar
  14. Muthen, L., & Muthen, B. (1998). Mplus. The comprehensive modeling program for applied researchers. Los Angeles: Muthen and Muthen.Google Scholar
  15. Nobes, G., Moore, D., Martin, A., Clifford, B., Butterworth, G., Panagiotaki, G., & Siegal, M. (2003). Children’s understanding of the Earth in a multicultural community: Mental models or fragments of knowledge? Developmental Science, 6, 74–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Nussbaum, J. (1979). Children’s conception of the Earth as a cosmic body: A cross-age study. Science Education, 63, 83–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Rolfhus, E., & Ackerman, P. (1999). Assessing individual differences in knowledge: Knowledge, intelligence and related traits. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 511–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. (1996). Mental models of the Earth, sun, and moon: Indian children’s cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Schnotz, W., Vosniadou, S., & Carretero, M. (Eds.) (1999). New perspectives on conceptual change. Amsterdam: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  20. Siegal, M., Butterworth, G., & Newcombe, P. (2004). Culture and children’s cosmology. Developmental Science, 7, 308–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Sneider, C., & Pulos, S. (1983). Children’s cosmographies: Understanding the Earth’s shape and gravity. Science Education, 67, 205–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Spelke, E. (1991). Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget’s theory. In S. Carey, & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 133–169). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Toomela, A. (1999). Drawing development: Stages in the representation of a cube and cylinder. Child Development, 26, 234–247.Google Scholar
  24. Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: A study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 234–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Vosniadou, S. (1994a). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 45–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Vosniadou, S. (1994b). Universal and culture-specific properties of children’s mental models of the Earth. In L. A. Hirschfeld, & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 412–429). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Vosniadou, S. (2003). Exploring the relationships between conceptual change and intentional learning. In G. Sinatra, & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 377–406). Mahwah, New Jersey and London, UK: LEA.Google Scholar
  28. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. (1992). Mental models of the Earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night cycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Vosniadou, S., Skopleiti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2004). Modes of knowing and ways of reasoning in elementary astronomy. Cognitive Development, 19, 203–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of EducationUniversity of Tartu, Estonian Centre of Behavioural and Health SciencesTartuEstonia
  2. 2.Faculty of EducationUniversity of TartuTartuEstonia

Personalised recommendations