Advertisement

Examining the visual attention span deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia

  • Nga Ting Chen
  • Mo Zheng
  • Connie Suk-Han Ho
Article

Abstract

The present study examines the visual attention span deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Twenty-five Cantonese-speaking Chinese dyslexic children (DD group) were compared with 25 chronological-age-matched (CA) controls and another 25 reading-level-matched (RL) controls on their performance in visual attention span, reading, and reading-related cognitive tasks. Results of analysis of variance showed that the DD group performed significantly worse than the CA group in most measures and less well than the RL group in the visual attention span character task. 68 and 52% of the dyslexic participants showed deficits in at least one visual attention span measure when compared with the CA group and the RL group respectively. Results of multiple regression analysis also showed that the composite score of visual attention span significantly predicted Chinese word reading accuracy, word and text reading fluency even after controlling for age, IQ, orthographic skills, and rapid naming. The present findings suggest that visual attention span deficit may be associated with developmental dyslexia in Chinese. The role of visual attention span for word and text reading in Chinese is above and beyond several known reading-related skills.

Keywords

Visual attention span Developmental dyslexia Chinese Cognitive deficit 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5). Philadelphia: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104(2), 198–230.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Breznitz, S. (1997). The effect of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 287–299.Google Scholar
  5. Casco, C., Tressoldi, P. E., & Dellantonio, A. (1998). Visual selective attention and reading efficiency are related in children. Cortex, 34, 531–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(6), 510–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Compton, D. L., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Are RAN-and phonological awareness-deficits additive in children with reading disabilities? Dyslexia, 7(3), 125–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471–479.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 36, 109–123.Google Scholar
  11. Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 814–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frith, U. (1997). Brain, mind and behaviour in dyslexia.Google Scholar
  13. Gardner, M. F. (1996). Test of visual-perceptual skills (non-motor): Revised manual. New York: Psychological and Educational Publications.Google Scholar
  14. Germano, G. D., Reilhac, C., Capellini, S. A., & Valdois, S. (2014). The phonological and visual basis of developmental dyslexia in Brazilian Portuguese reading children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goswami, U. (2014). Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: Three challenges for resesarch. Nature Reviews Neuroscience.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3836.Google Scholar
  16. Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45(7), 855–863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ho, C. S.-H. (2014). Preschool predictors of dyslexia status in Chinese first graders with high or low familial risk. Reading and Writing, 27, 1673–1701.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9515-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D., Chung, K., Tsang, S., Lee, S., & Cheng, R. (2007). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing for primary school students-[HKT-P (II)]. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.Google Scholar
  20. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 543–553.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D., Tsang, S.-M., Lee, S.-H., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ho, C. S.-H., Ng, T.-T., & Ng, W.-K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35(3), 849–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hu, C.-F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(1), 55–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (1992). Phonological deficits in dyslexia: A “sound” reappraisal of the verbal deficit hypothesis? In N. N. Singh & I. L. Beale (Eds.), Progress in learning disabilities (pp. 270–301). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lallier, M., Donnadieu, S., & Valdois, S. (2013). Developmental dyslexia: Exploring how much phonological and visual attention span disorders are linked to simultaneous auditory processing deficits. Annals of Dyslexia, 63(2), 97–116.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-012-0074-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppanen, P. H. T., Lohvansuu, K., et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Law, N., Ki, W. W., Chung, A. L. S., Ko, P. Y., & Lam, H. C. (1998). Children’s stroke sequence errors in writing Chinese characters. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10(3–5), 267–292.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008091730338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Leung, M. T., & Lee, A. W. Y. (2002). The Hong Kong Corpus of primary school Chinese. Paper presented at the 9th meeting of the international clinical phonetics and linguistics association. Hong Kong.Google Scholar
  29. Li, W.-S., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2011). Lexical tone awareness among Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Child Language, 38, 793–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Liu, D. (2014). Visual-spatial attention and its relationship with reading. In K. K. H. Chung, K. C. P. Yuen, & D. M. Mclnerney (Eds.), Understanding developmental disorder of auditory processing, language and literacy across languages: International perspectives (pp. 169–188). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  31. Lobier, M. A., Peyrin, C., Pichat, C., Le Bas, J.-F., & Valdois, S. (2014). Visual processing of multiple elements in the dyslexic brain: Evidence for a superior parietal dysfunction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00479.Google Scholar
  32. Lobier, M., & Valdois, S. (2015). Visual attention deficits in developmental dyslexia cannot be ascribed solely to poor reading experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3836-c1.Google Scholar
  33. Lobier, M., Zoubrinetzky, R., & Valdois, S. (2012). The visual attention span deficit in dyslexia is visual and not verbal. Cortex, 48(6), 768–773.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.09.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., & Holopainen, L. (2004). Dyslexia in highly orthographically regular Finnish. In I. Smythe, J. Everatt, & R. Slater (Eds.), International book of dyslexia: A cross-language comparison and practice guide (pp. 81–91). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: Varability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 347–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pammer, K., Lavis, R., Hansen, P., & Cornelissen, P. L. (2004). Symbol-string sensitivity and children’s reading. Brain and Language, 89, 601–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Peyrin, C., Lallier, M., Démonet, J. F., Pernet, C., Baciu, M., Le Bas, J. F., et al. (2012). Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: FMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain and Language, 120(3), 381–394.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2007). The contribution of phonological awareness and visual attention in early reading and spelling. Dyslexia, 13, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Prado, C., Dubois, M., & Valdois, S. (2007). The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search: Impact of the visual attention span. Vision Research, 47, 2521–2530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1996). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  41. Saksida, A., Iannuzzi, S., Bogliotti, C., Chaix, Y., Demonet, J.-F., Bricout, L., et al. (2016). Phonological skills, visual attention span, and visual stress in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1503–1516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(1), 37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stanovich, K., Feeman, D., & Cunningham, A. (1983). The development of the relation between letter-naming speed and reading ability. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21(3), 199–202.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., Gottardo, A., Chiappe, P., & Sidhu, R. (1997). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Differences in phonological and orthographic coding. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia (pp. 115–141). Mahwah: LEA.Google Scholar
  45. Stenneken, P., Egetemeir, J., Schulte-Korne, G., Muller, H. J., Schneider, W. X., & Finke, K. (2011). Slow perceptual processing at the core of developmental dyslexia: A parameter-based assessment of visual attention. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Valdois, S., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Reilhac, C., N’guyen-Morel, M.-A., Guinet, E., et al. (2011). A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex, 47(10), 1197–1218.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., & Tainturier, M.-J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Valdois, S., Lassus-Sangosse, D., & Lobier, M. (2012). Impaired letter-string processing in developmental dyslexia: What visual-to-phonology code mapping disorder? Dyslexia, 18(2), 77–93.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Valdois, S., Peyrin, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Lallier, M., Démonet, J.-F., & Kandel, S. (2014). Dyslexia in a French–Spanish bilingual girl: Behavioural and neural modulations following a visual attention span intervention. Cortex, 53, 120–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 57–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wolf, M. (1982). The word-retrieval process and reading in children and aphasics. Children’s Language, 3, 437.Google Scholar
  53. Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 415–438.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wolf, M., O’Rourke, A., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 15(1–2), 43–72.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013816320290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wong, A. M.-Y., Ho, C. S.-H., Au, T. K.-F., Kidd, J. C., Ng, A. K.-H., Yip, L. P.-W., et al. (2015). (Dis)connections between specific language impairment and dyslexia in Chinese. Reading and Writing.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9546-3.Google Scholar
  57. Yap, R., & van der Leij, A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics: An automatic decoding deficit? Reading and Writing, 5, 261–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2014). What are the early indicators of persistent word reading difficulties among Chinese readers in elementary grades? Dyslexia.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1471.Google Scholar
  59. Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Wong, Y.-K., Chan, D. W.-O., Chung, K. K.-H., & Lo, L.-Y. (2013). Longitudinal predictors of Chinese word reading and spelling among elementary grade students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(06), 1245–1277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., Dufau, S., & Grainger, J. (2010). Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: What purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia? Developmental Science, 13(4), F8–F14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Korne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of Hong KongPok Fu LamHong Kong

Personalised recommendations