Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 31, Issue 4, pp 1017–1038 | Cite as

Handwriting in signing deaf middle-school students and relationship with text composition and spelling

  • Denis Alamargot
  • Marie-France Morin
  • Érika Simard-Dupuis
Article

Abstract

We set out to (i) assess the handwriting skills of signing deaf students, and (ii) examine the extent to which their text composition and spelling performances are linked to their handwriting efficiency. We asked 15 prelingually and profoundly deaf middle-school students (M = 15.18 years), all sign-language users, and a group of hearing students matched on chronological age (M = 15.32 years) to write the letters of the alphabet and their firstname and surname from memory as a handwriting assessment, and to compose a text describing their bedroom. Results showed that even though the deaf students formed legible letters in both handwriting tasks, they spent more time producing the alphabet letters, and composed shorter texts that were less fluent and contained more phonologically inaccurate spelling errors. Analysis of correlations between handwriting and text production (including spelling) measures revealed strong and significant relationships for the deaf students. This study showed that handwriting difficulties, in terms of executing the letters of their firstname and surname, as well as retrieving the letters of the alphabet from memory, persist in 15-year-old signing deaf students, and represent a constraint during text production, impairing spelling as well as text content richness and word fluency.

Keywords

Deafness Handwriting Text composition Spelling Legibility Fluency 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the schools that helped us to test their pupils, the two specialist institutions for the deaf in the French cities of Poitiers (IRJS) and Bordeaux (INJS) and the Vienne département’s education service, which facilitated our research team’s interventions in the different schools. We would also like to thank Elizabeth Portier for translating and revising the English style of this article.

Funding

This research received financial support from the French National Research Agency (ANR)’s Dynamics of Orthographic Processing (DyTO) project; the state/region planning contract (CPER) for Poitou–Charentes (France); and the Research chair in reading and writing learning in young children (CREALEC; University of Sherbrooke, Quebec, Canada).

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All procedures in this study involving human participants were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national review board, the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards, and France’s 1988 Huriet Act on the bioethical protection of persons involved in human experimentations.

References

  1. Aaron, P., Keetay, V., Boyd, M., Palmatier, S., & Wacks, J. (1998). Spelling without phonology: A study of deaf and hearing children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10, 1–22.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007917929226.Google Scholar
  2. Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills in primary—and intermediate—grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 478–508.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.478.Google Scholar
  3. Alamargot, D. (1997). Processus de récupération et d’organisation dans l’activité de rédaction de texte. Rôle du développement des connaissances référentielles. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Poitiers, France.Google Scholar
  4. Alamargot, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2001). Through the models of writing. Dordrecht-Boston-London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and Pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behaviour Research Methods, 38(2), 287–299.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192780.Google Scholar
  6. Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007a). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In M. Torrance, L. V. Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 13–29). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  7. Alamargot, D., Lambert, E., Thebault, C., & Dansac, C. (2007b). Text composition by deaf and hearing middle-school students: The role of working memory. Reading and Writing, 20, 333–360.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9033-y.Google Scholar
  8. Alamargot, D., & Morin, M.-F. (2015). Does handwriting on a tablet screen affect students’ graphomotor execution? A comparison between Grades 2 and 9. Human Movement Science, 44, 32–41.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.011.Google Scholar
  9. Alamargot, D., Morin, M.-F., Pontart, V., Maffre, L., Flouret, L., & Simard-Dupuis, É. (2014). Les enfants dyslexiques ont-ils des difficultés graphomotrices? Approche Neuropsychologique des Apprentissages chez l’Enfant - ANAE, 128, 59–67.Google Scholar
  10. Albertini, J. A., & Schley, S. (2011). Writing: Characteristics, instruction, and assessment. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (2nd ed., pp. 130–143). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Allman, T. M. (2002). Patterns of spelling in young deaf and hard of hearing students. American Annals of the Deaf, 147, 46–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Amundson, S. (1995). Evaluation tool of children’s handwriting. Homer, AK: OT Kids.Google Scholar
  13. Antia, S. D., Reed, S., & Kreimeyer, K. H. (2005). Written language of deaf and hard-of-hearing students in public schools. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10(3), 244–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Apel, K., & Masterson, J. J. (2015). Comparing the spelling and reading abilities of students with cochlear implants and students with typical hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education (Advance Access published), 20, 125–135.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/env002.Google Scholar
  15. Arfé, B., Nicolini, F., & E. Pozzebon (2014). The influence of verbal working memory on writing skills in children with hearing loss. In B. Arfe, J. Dockrell, & V. Berninger (Eds.), Writing development in children with hearing loss, dyslexia, or oral language problems. Implications for assessment and instruction (pp. 85–99). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Arfé, B., & Perondi, I. (2008). Deaf and hearing students’ referential strategies in writing: What referential cohesion tells us about deaf students’ literacy development. First Language, 28(4), 355–374.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723708091043.Google Scholar
  17. Bara, F., & Gentaz, E. (2010). Apprendre à tracer les lettres: Une revue critique. Psychologie française, 55, 129–144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2010.01.001.Google Scholar
  18. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 57, 289–300.Google Scholar
  19. Berninger, V. W., & Alsdorf, B. (1989). Are there errors in error analysis? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 7, 209–222.  https://doi.org/10.1177/073428298900700303.Google Scholar
  20. Berninger, V. W., & Amtmann, D. (2004). Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: Research into practice. In L. Swanson, K. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning disabilities (pp. 345–363). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  21. Berninger, V. W., Mizokawa, D. T., & Bragg, R. (1991). Theory-based diagnosis and remediation of writing disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 57–79.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-4405(91)90016-K.Google Scholar
  22. Bishop, D. V. M. (1983). Comprehension of English syntax by profoundly deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(3), 415–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Boutla, M., Supalla, T., Newport, E., & Bavelier, D. (2004). Short-term memory span: Insights from sign language. Nature Neuroscience, 7(9), 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Bowers, L., McCarthy, J. H., Schwarz, I., Dostal, H., & Wolbers, K. (2014). Examination of the spelling skills of middle school students who are deaf and hard of hearing. The Volta Review, 114(1), 29–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Burman, D., Nunes, T., & Evans, D. (2007). Writing profiles of deaf children taught through British sign language. Deafness and Education International, 9(1), 2–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J. N., & Fayol, M. (1990). Temporal management of short text writing by children and adults. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 10(5), 513–538.Google Scholar
  27. Chartrel, E., & Vinter, A. (2006). Rôle des informations visuelles dans la production de lettres cursives chez l’enfant et l’adulte. L’Année Psychologique, 106, 43–64.  https://doi.org/10.4074/S0003503306001047.Google Scholar
  28. Chesnet, D., & Alamargot, D. (2005). Analyse en temps réel des activités oculaires et grapho-motrices du scripteur: Intérêt du dispositif “Eye and Pen”. L’Année Psychologique, 105(3), 477–520.  https://doi.org/10.3406/psy.2005.29706.Google Scholar
  29. Christensen, C. A. (2005). The role of orthographic-motor integration in the production of creative and well-structured written text for students in secondary school. Educational Psychology, 25(5), 441–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Chuy, M., Alamargot, D., & Passerault, J.-M. (2012). Interference with prior knowledge while writing from sources: Effects of working memory capacity. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 815–828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Colombo, L., Arfé, B., & Bronte, T. (2012). The influence of phonological mechanisms in written spelling of profoundly deaf children. Reading and Writing, 25, 2021–2038.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9343-6.Google Scholar
  32. Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., & Barnett, J. (2005). The slow handwriting of undergraduate students constrains overall performance in exam essays. Educational Psychology, 25, 99–107.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341042000294912.Google Scholar
  33. Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., Walter, K., & Critten, S. (2012). Predicting the quality of composition and written language bursts from oral language, spelling, and handwriting skills in children with specific language impairment. Written Communication, 29(3), 278–302.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0741088312451109.Google Scholar
  34. Conte, M. P., Rampelli, L. P., & Volterra, V. (1996). Deaf children and the construction of written texts. In C. Pontecorvo & M. Orsolini (Eds.), Children’s early text construction (pp. 303–319). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  35. Conway, C. M., Karpicke, J., Anaya, E. M., Henning, S. C., Kronenberger, W. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2011). Nonverbal cognition in deaf children following cochlear implantation: Motor sequencing disturbances mediate language delays. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(2), 237–254.  https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549869.Google Scholar
  36. Daigle, D., & Dubuisson, C. (1998). Que peut-on conclure des recherches portant sur l’écriture? In C. Dubuisson & D. Daigle (Eds.), Lecture, écriture et surdité: Visions actuelles et nouvelles perspectives (pp. 131–151). Montreal: Les Éditions Logiques.Google Scholar
  37. DeBruyn, I., Smith, R., & Berninger, V. (1985). Visual and linguistic correlates of beginning skill. Paper Presented at the National Association of School Psychologists, Las Vegas, NVGoogle Scholar
  38. Dodd, B. (1980). The spelling abilities of profoundly pre-linguistically deaf children. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 423–443). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  39. Dostal, H. M., & Wolbers, K. A. (2014). Developing language and writing skills of deaf and hard of hearing students: A simultaneous approach. Literacy Research and Instruction, 53(3), 245–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Doutriaux, F., & Lepez, R. (1980). Test de Niveau d’Orthographe (TNO). Paris, France: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
  41. Ehrich, V., & Koster, C. (1983). Discourse organization and sentence form: The structure of room description in Dutch. Discourse Processes, 6, 169–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ellis, A. W. (1988). Normal writing processes and peripheral acquired dysgraphias. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 99–127.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968808402084.Google Scholar
  43. Fayol, M., & Miret, A. (2005). Écrire, orthographier et rédiger des textes. Psychologie française, 50, 391–402.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2005.05.008.Google Scholar
  44. Geers, A. E., & Hayes, H. (2011). Reading, writing, and phonological processing skills of adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear and Hearing, 32(1), 49–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Geers, A. E., & Moog, J. (1989). Factors predictive of the development of literacy in profoundly hearing-impaired adolescents. Volta Review, 91, 69–86.Google Scholar
  46. Goldberg, H. R., & Lederberg, A. R. (2015). Acquisition of the alphabetic principle in deaf and hard-of-hearing preschoolers: The role of phonology in letter-sound learning. Reading and Writing, 28(4), 509–525.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9535-y.Google Scholar
  47. Graham, S., Berninger, V., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.170.Google Scholar
  48. Hanson, V. L., Shankweiler, D., & Fisher, F. W. (1983). Determinants of spelling ability in deaf and hearing adults: Access to linguistic structure. Cognition, 14(3), 323–344.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90009-4.Google Scholar
  49. Harris, M., & Terlektsi, E. (2010). Reading and spelling abilities of deaf adolescents with cochlear implants and hearing aids. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(1), 24–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Hayes, H., Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2011). Spelling of deaf children who use cochlear implants. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 522–540.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2010.528480.Google Scholar
  51. Jones, D., & Christensen, C. A. (1999). Relationship between automaticity in handwriting and students’ ability to generate written text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 44–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kelly, L. P. (1987). The influence of syntactic anomalies on the writing processes of a deaf college student. In A. Matsuhashi (Ed.), Writing in real time: Modeling production processes (pp. 161–196). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
  53. Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predictors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11(3), 273–288.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enj037.Google Scholar
  54. Leybaert, J., & Alegria, J. (1995). Spelling development in deaf and hearing children. Evidence for use of morpho-phonological regularities in French. Reading and Writing, 7, 89–109.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026949.Google Scholar
  55. Leybaert, J., Alegria, J., Hage, C., & Charlier, B. (1998). The effect of exposure to phonetically augmented lipspeech in the prelingual deaf. In R. Campbell, B. Dood, & D. Burnham (Eds.), Hearing by eye II: Advances in the psychology of speechreading and auditory- visual speech (pp. 283–301). Hove, UK: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  56. Leybaert, J., & Lechat, J. (2001). Variability in deaf children’s spelling: The effect of language experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 554–562.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.554.Google Scholar
  57. Lichtenstein, E. H. (1998). The relationships between reading processes and English skills of deaf college students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3(2), 80–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Marschark, M., Mouradian, V., & Halas, M. (1994). Discourse rules in the language productions of deaf and hearing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57(1), 89–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Marschark, M., Rhoten, C., & Fabich, M. (2007). Effects of cochlear implants on children’s reading and academic achievement. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 269–282.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm013.Google Scholar
  60. Matsuhashi, A. (1982). Explorations in the real-time production of written discourse. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language and structure of written discourse (pp. 269–289). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  61. Mayer, C. (2010). The demands of writing and the deaf writer. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (Vol. 2, pp. 144–155). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Mayer, C., & Moskos, E. (1998). Deaf children learning to spell. Research in the Teaching of English, 33, 158–180.Google Scholar
  63. Mayer, C., Watson, L., Archbold, S., Yen Ng, Z., & Mulla, I. (2016). Reading and writing skills of deaf pupils with cochlear implants. Deafness & Education International, 18(2), 71–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. McCarney, D., Peters, L., Jackson, S., Thomas, M., & Kirby, A. (2013). Does poor handwriting conceal literacy potential in primary school children? International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 60(2), 105–118.  https://doi.org/10.1080/1034912X.2013.786561.Google Scholar
  65. McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 299–325.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01464076.Google Scholar
  66. McCutchen, D. (2011). From novice to expert: Implications of language skills and writing-relevant knowledge for memory during the development of writing skill. Journal of Writing Research, 3, 51–68. Published on line under Creative Commons Attribution.Google Scholar
  67. Medwell, J., Strand, S., & Wray, D. (2007). The role of handwriting in composing for Y2 children. Journal of Reading, Writing and Literacy, 2(1), 11–21.Google Scholar
  68. Medwell, J., Strand, S., & Wray, D. (2009). The links between handwriting and composing for Y6 children. Cambridge Journal of Education, 39(3), 329–344.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03057640903103728.Google Scholar
  69. Medwell, J., & Wray, D. (2014). Handwriting automaticity: The search for performance thresholds. Language and Education, 28(1), 34–51.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09500782.2013.763819.Google Scholar
  70. Morin, M.-F., Bara, F., & Alamargot, D. (2017). Apprentissage de la graphomotricité à l’école: Quelles acquisitions? Quelles pratiques? Quels outils? Scientia Paedagogica Experimentalis, 54(1–2), 47–84.Google Scholar
  71. Morin, M.-F., Lavoie, N., & Montésinos-Gelet, I. (2012). The effects of manuscript, cursive, or manuscript/cursive styles on writing development in Grade 2. Language and Literacy, 14(1), 110–124. Published on line under Creative Commons Attribution.Google Scholar
  72. Musselman, C., & Szanto, G. (1998). The written language of deaf adolescents: Patterns of performances. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3(3), 245–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Nelson, N. W., & Crumpton, T. (2015). Reading, writing, and spoken language assessment profiles for students who are deaf and hard of hearing compared with students with language learning disabilities. Topic in Language Disorders, 35(2), 157–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Padden, C. A. (1993). Lessons to be learned from the young deaf orthographer. Linguistics and Education, 5(1), 7186.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0898-5898(05)80005-1.Google Scholar
  75. Plisson, A., Berthiaume, R., & Daigle, D. (2010). Compétence orthographique chez l’élève dyslexique et chez l’élève sourd: eEtude comparative. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistic/Revue Canadienne de Linguistique Appliquée, 13, 165–187.Google Scholar
  76. Pontart, V., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lambert, E., Morisset, P., Flouret, L., & Alamargot, D. (2013). Influence of handwriting skills during spelling in primary and lower secondary grades. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 19.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00818.Google Scholar
  77. Puranik, C. S., & AlOtaiba, S. (2012). Examining the contribution of handwriting and spelling to written expression in kindergarten children. Reading and Writing, 25, 1523–1546.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9331-x.Google Scholar
  78. Quigley, S., & King, C. (1980). Syntactic performance of hearing impaired and normal hearing individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 329–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rogers, W. T., Leslie, P. T., Clarke, B. R., Booth, J. A., & Horvath, A. (1978). Academic achievement of hearing impaired students: Comparisons among selected subpopulations. B.C. Journal of Special Education, 2, 183–213.Google Scholar
  80. Savage, G. A. (1978). Handwriting of the deaf and hard of hearing. Canadian Society of Forensic Science, 11(1), 114.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00085030.1978.10756844.Google Scholar
  81. Schlumberger, E., Narbona, J., & Manrique, M. (2004). Non-verbal development of children with deafness with and without cochlear implants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46, 599606.  https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549869.Google Scholar
  82. Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika, 73, 751–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Singer, B. D., & Bashir, A. S. (2004). Developmental variations in written composition skills. In C. A. Stone, E. R. Silliman, B. J. Ehrren, & K. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 559–582). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  84. Singleton, J. L., Morgan, D., DiGello, E., Wiles, J., & Rivers, R. (2004). Vocabulary use by low, moderate, and high ASL-proficient writers compared to hearing ESL and monolingual speakers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9(1), 86–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Sutcliffe, A., Dowker, A., & Campbell, R. (1999). Deaf children’s spelling. Does it show sensitivity to phonology? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4, 111–123.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/4.2.111.Google Scholar
  86. Svirsky, M. A., Robbins, A. M., Iler Kirk, K., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2000). Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychological Science, 11(2), 153–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Human Movement Science, 10, 165191.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(91)90003.Google Scholar
  88. Van Staden, A., & Le Roux, N. A. (2010). The efficacy of fingerspell coding and visual imaging techniques in improving the spelling proficiency of deaf signing elementary-phase children: A South-African case study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22, 581–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Wakefield, P. (2006). Deaf children’s approaches to spelling—difficulties, strategies and teaching techniques. Deafness and Education International, 8(4), 174–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  91. Wilbur, R. B. (2000). The use of ASL to support the development of English and literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 81–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Williams, C., & Mayer, C. (2015). Writing in young deaf children. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 630–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Denis Alamargot
    • 1
  • Marie-France Morin
    • 2
  • Érika Simard-Dupuis
    • 2
  1. 1.Laboratoire Cognitions Humaine et Artificielle (CHArt- EA 4004)Université Paris-Est, UPECCréteilFrance
  2. 2.Chaire de Recherche sur l’Apprentissage de la Lecture et de l’ECriture chez le jeune enfant (CREALEC), Faculté d’EducationUniversité de SherbrookeQuébecCanada

Personalised recommendations