Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 26, Issue 6, pp 991–1008 | Cite as

Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution

Article

Abstract

It is commonly assumed that children with dyslexia are slower at handwriting than other children. However, evidence of slow handwriting in children with dyslexia is very mixed. Thirty-one children with dyslexia, aged 9 years, were compared to both age-matched children and younger spelling-ability matched children. Participants completed an alphabet-writing task and a composition task on the surface of a digital writing tablet. Children with dyslexia wrote the same amount of letters per minute in the alphabet task but wrote fewer words per minute when composing their texts than children of the same age. Crucially, no differences were found between children with dyslexia and their same age peers for speed of handwriting execution, measured by the tablet, when writing the alphabet or composing their texts. However, children with dyslexia were found to pause within their compositions as often as the spelling ability matched group. Thus handwriting execution is not impaired in children with dyslexia. The slow writing that is typical of children with dyslexia is due to pausing more often when composing and is related to spelling ability. This may reflect processing problems in response to high cognitive load through having to contend with spelling and composing concurrently.

Keywords

Composing Dyslexia Handwriting Speed Writing 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a studentship from The Waterloo Foundation. Special thanks go to the children and schools that participated in this study. This research was supported by a studentship from The Waterloo Foundation and Oxford Brookes University awarded to Emma Sumner.

References

  1. Abbott, R., & Berninger, V. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary and intermediate grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478–508. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behaviour Research Methods, 38, 287–299. doi: 10.3758/BF03192780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In M. Torrance, L. van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (Vol. 20, pp. 13–29). Oxford, United Kingdom: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Alamargot, D., Plane, S., Lambert, E., & Chesnet, D. (2009). Using eye and pen movements to trace the development of writing expertise: Case studies of a 7th, 9th and 12th grader, a graduate student, and professional writer. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 853–888. doi: 10.1007/s11145-009-9191-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barnett, A., Henderson, S. E., Scheib, B., & Schulz, J. (2007). DASH detailed assessment of speed of handwriting. London, United Kingdom: Pearson.Google Scholar
  6. Berninger, V. W., Cartwright, A., Yates, C., Swanson, H. L., & Abbott, R. (1994). Developmental skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades: Shared and unique functional systems. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 161–196. doi: 10.1007/BF01026911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008a). Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: Under-recognized and under-treated. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.11.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berninger, V. W., & Rutberg, J. (1992). Relationship of finger function to beginning writing: Application to diagnosis of writing disabilities. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 155–172. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1992.tb14993.x.Google Scholar
  9. Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of development of skilled writing (pp. 57–81). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
  10. Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R. D., Begay, K., Coleman, K. B., Curtin, G., et al. (2002). Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: Implications for the simple view of writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 291–304. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Berninger, V. W., Winn, W. D., Stock, P., Abbott, R. D., Eschen, K., Lin, S.-J., et al. (2008b). Tier 3 specialized writing instruction for students with dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 95–129. doi: 10.1007/s11145-007-9066-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Berninger, V. W., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in developing writing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 257–280. doi: 10.1007/BF01027151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower order skills to the written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 175–196. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. de Jong, O., & Messbauer, V. (2011). Orthographic context and the acquisition of orthographic knowledge in normal and dyslexic readers. Dyslexia, 17, 107–122. doi: 10.1002/dys.427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. De Kleine, E., & Verway, W. B. (2009). Motor learning and chunking in dyslexia. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 41, 331–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De La Paz, S., & Graham, S. (1997). Effects of dictation and advanced planning instruction on the composing of students with writing and learning problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 203–222. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dockrell, J. E., Lindsay, G., & Connelly, V. (2009). The impact of specific language impairment on adolescents’ written text. Exceptional Children, 75, 427–446.Google Scholar
  19. Dockrell, J. E., Lindsay, G., Connelly, V., & Mackie, C. (2007). Constraints in the production of written text in children with specific language impairments. Exceptional Children, 73, 147–164.Google Scholar
  20. Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). British abilities scales second edition: Administration and scoring manual. Berkshire, United Kingdom: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
  21. Fitzgerald, J., & Shanahan, T. (2000). Reading and writing relations and their development. Educational Psychologist, 35, 39–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goldup, W. (2000). Developing writing skills. In J. Townend & M. Turner (Eds.), Dyslexia in practice: A guide for teachers (pp. 131–154). London, United Kingdom: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Graham, S. (1990). The role of production factors in learning disabled students’ compositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 781–791. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Graham, S., Harris, K. R., MacArthur, C. A., & Schwartz, S. (1991). Writing and writing instruction for students with learning disabilities: Review of a research program. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 89–114. doi: 10.2307/1510517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gregg, N. G., Coleman, C., Davis, M., & Chalk, J. C. (2007). Timed essay writing: Implications for high-stakes test. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 306–318. doi: 10.1177/00222194070400040201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hatcher, J., Snowling, M. J., & Griffiths, Y. M. (2002). Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 119–133. doi: 10.1348/000709902158801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Henderson, S., Sugden, D., & Barnett, A. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children: Second edition: manual. London, United Kingdom: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  29. Iversen, S., Berg, K., Ellertsen, B., & Tonnessen, F. E. (2005). Motor coordination difficulties in a municipality group and in a clinical sample of poor readers. Dyslexia, 11, 217–231. doi: 10.1002/dys.297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kandel, S., Alvarez, C. J., & Vallee, N. (2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 18–31. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kandel, S., Peereman, R., Grosjacques, G., & Fayol, M. (2011). For a psycholinguistic model of handwriting production: Testing the syllable-bigram controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1310–1322. doi: 10.1037/a0023094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kaplan, B., Wilson, B., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00010-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kemp, N., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2009). Phonological and orthographic spelling in high-functioning adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15, 105–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). Defining dyslexia, comorbidity, teachers’ knowledge of language and reading: A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Maggio, S., Lété, B., Chenu, F., Jisa, H., & Fayol, M. (2011). Tracking the mind during writing: Immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pauses and writing rate. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9348-1.
  36. Martlew, M. (1992). Handwriting and spelling: Dyslexic children’s abilities compared with children of the same chronological age and younger children of the same spelling level. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 375–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 299–324. doi:  10.1007/BF01464076.
  38. Menghini, D., Hagberg, G. E., Caltagirone, C., Petrosini, L., & Vicari, S. (2006). Implicit learning deficits in dyslexic adults: An fMRI scan. NeuroImage, 33, 1218–1226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mortimore, T., & Crozier, W. R. (2006). Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 31, 235–251. doi: 10.1080/03075070600572173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47, 117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Olive, T. (2010). Methods, techniques, and tools for the on-line study of the writing process. In N. L. Mertens (Ed.), Writing: processes, tools and techniques (pp. 1–18). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  42. Ott, P. (2007). Teaching children with dyslexia: A practical guide. Oxford, United Kingdom: Routledge.Google Scholar
  43. Rose, J. (2009). Identifying and teaching children and young people with dyslexia and literacy difficulties. London, United Kingdom: Department for Children Schools and Families.Google Scholar
  44. Rust, J. (1996). The manual of the Wechsler objective language dimensions (WOLD) (UK ed.). London, United Kingdom: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  45. Shanahan, T. (2006). Relations among oral language, reading and writing development. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 171–183). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  46. Snowling, M. J. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 294–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sovik, N., & Arntzen, O. (1986). A comparative study of the writing/spelling performances of ‘normal’, dyslexic, and dysgraphic children. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 1, 85–101. doi: 10.1080/0885625860010201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sterling, C., Farmer, M., Riddick, B., Morgan, S., & Matthews, C. (1998). Adult dyslexic writing. Dyslexia, 4, 1–15. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(199803)4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sumner, E., Connelly, V., & Barnett, A. (in press). Dyslexia and expressive writing in English. In B. Arfé, J. E. Dockrell, & V. W. Berninger (Eds.), Writing development and instruction in children with hearing, speech and oral language difficulties. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Wagner, R. K., Puranik, C. S., Foorman, B., Foster, E., Wilson, L. G., Tschinkel, E., et al. (2011). Modelling the development of written language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 203–220. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9266-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wengelin, A. (2007). The word-level focus in text production by adults with reading and writing difficulties. In M. W. Torrance, L. van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (Vol. 20, pp. 67–82). Oxford, United Kingdom: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  52. Wengelin, A., & Stromqvist, S. (2000). Discourse level writing in dyslexics: methods, results, and implications for diagnosis. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 25, 22–28. doi: 10.1080/140154300750045876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Emma Sumner
    • 1
  • Vincent Connelly
    • 1
  • Anna L. Barnett
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyOxford Brookes UniversityOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations