Reading and Writing

, Volume 26, Issue 2, pp 225–239 | Cite as

Why are written picture naming latencies (not) longer than spoken naming?

Article

Abstract

The comparison between spoken and handwritten production in picture naming tasks represents an important source of information for building models of cognitive processes involved in writing. Studies using this methodology systematically reported longer latencies for handwritten than for spoken production. To uncover the origin of this difference across modalities, we compared the latencies of spoken picture naming and two written picture naming conditions: one in which the participants could see and monitor their handwriting (visible-condition), and one in which they could not monitor their production (masked-condition). Previously reported differences between spoken and handwritten naming latencies were replicated in the standard visible-condition. By contrast, production latencies were faster in the written masked-condition than in the visible-condition and did not differ from spoken production latencies. These results suggest that longer handwriting latencies, in comparison with speaking latencies, are due to the delayed onset in handwriting in conditions where the sheet is visible. The implications of these results on both written production models and experimental methods are discussed.

Keywords

Handwritten visible- and masked-conditions Latencies Spoken word production Written word production 

References

  1. Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 287–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alario, F.-X., & Ferrand, L. (1999). A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31, 531–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alario, F.-X., & Moscoso del Prado Martin, F. (2010). On the origin of the “cumulative semantic inhibition” effect. Memory & Cognition, 38, 57–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A Practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boersma, P., & Weenik, D. (2007). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. Institute of Phonetic Sciences of the University of Amsterdam. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org.
  7. Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Méot, A., & Fayol, M. (2002). The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 89–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonin, P., Collay, S., Fayol, M., & Meot, A. (2005). Attentional strategic control over sublexical and lexical processing in written spelling to dictation in adults. Memory and Cognition, 33, 59–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bonin, P., & Fayol, M. (2000). Writing words from pictures: What representations are activated, and when? Memory & Cognition, 28, 677–689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bonin, P., & Fayol, M. (2002). Frequency effects in the written and spoken production of homophonic picture names. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 289–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Chalard, M. (2001a). Age of acquisition and word frequency in written picture naming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 469–489.Google Scholar
  12. Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Gombert, J.-E. (1998). An experimental study of lexical access in the writing and naming of isolated words. International Journal of Psychology, 33, 269–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bonin, P., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2001b). Do phonological codes constrain the selection of orthographic codes in written picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 688–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Méot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 177–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Caramazza, A., & Miceli, G. (1990). The structure of graphemic representations. Cognition, 37, 243–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chateau, D., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Strategic effects in word naming: Examining the route-emphasis versus time-criterion accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 139–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ellis, A. W. (1988). Normal writing processes and peripheral acquired dysgraphias. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 99–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2006). Eye-hand interactions in tracing and drawing tasks. Human Movement Science, 25, 568–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Graham, S., & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospects from 1980 to 1994. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 7–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B., Romani, C., & Caramazza, A. (1990). Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 191–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hotopf, N. (1980). Slips of the pen. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling. New York, USA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  24. Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 115–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lambert, E., Kandel, S., Fayol, M., & Espéret, E. (2008). The effect of the number of syllables on handwriting production. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 859–883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.Google Scholar
  28. Lupker, S. J., Brown, P., & Colombo, L. (1997). Strategic control in a naming task: Changing routes or changing deadlines? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 570–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Luria, A. R. (1970). Traumatic aphasia: Its syndromes, Psychology, and Treatment. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). LEXIQUE 2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 516–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Perret, C., & Laganaro, M. (2012). Comparison of electrophysiological correlates of writing and speaking: A topographic ERP analysis. Brain Topography, 25, 64–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, http://www.R-project.org/.
  34. Roux, S., & Bonin, P. (2011). Processing in written naming: Evidence from the picture–picture interference paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes,. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.580162.Google Scholar
  35. Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Smyth, M. M., & Silvers, G. (1987). Functions of vision in the control of handwriting. Acta Psychologica, 65, 47–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tainturier, A.-J., & Rapp, B. (2001). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive: What deficits reveal about the human mind. Philadelphia, USA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  38. Taylor, T. E., & Lupker, S. J. (2001). Sequential effects in naming: a time-criterion account. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 117–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Zhang, Q., & Damian, M. F. (2010). Impact of phonology on the generation of handwritten responses: Evidence from picture-word interference tasks. Memory & Cognition, 38, 519–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.UFR Lettres, Sciences de l’Homme et des SociétésUniversité Paris, 13 Sorbonne Paris CitéVilletaneuseFrance
  2. 2.FAPSEGeneva UniversityGenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations