Reading and Writing

, Volume 25, Issue 2, pp 569–585 | Cite as

Linking the shapes of alphabet letters to their sounds: the case of Hebrew

Article

Abstract

Learning the sounds of letters is an important part of learning a writing system. Most previous studies of this process have examined English, focusing on variations in the phonetic iconicity of letter names as a reason why some letter sounds (such as that of b, where the sound is at the beginning of the letter’s name) are easier to learn than others (such as that of w, where the sound is not in the name). The present study examined Hebrew, where variations in the phonetic iconicity of letter names are minimal. In a study of 391 Israeli children with a mean age of 5 years, 10 months, we used multilevel models to examine the factors that are associated with knowledge of letter sounds. One set of factors involved letter names: Children sometimes attributed to a letter a consonant–vowel sound consisting of the first phonemes of the letter’s name. A second set of factors involved contrast: Children had difficulty when there was relatively little contrast in shape between one letter and others. Frequency was also important, encompassing both child-specific effects, such as a benefit for the first letter of a child’s forename, and effects that held true across children, such as a benefit for the first letters of the alphabet. These factors reflect general properties of human learning.

Keywords

Alphabet Hebrew Letter names Letter sounds 

References

  1. Bates, D. (2009). Computational methods for mixed models. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/vignettes/Theory.pdf.
  2. Bolozky, S. (1999). On the special status of the vowels a and e in Israeli Hebrew. Hebrew Studies, 40, 233–250.Google Scholar
  3. Bouchière, B., Ponce, C., & Foulin, J.-N. (2010). Développement de la connaissance des lettres capitales. Étude transversale chez les enfants français de trois à six ans [The development of uppercase letter knowledge. A cross-sectional study of French children aged three to six]. Psychologie Française, 55, 65–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ecalle, J. (2004). Les connaissances des lettres et l’écriture du prénom chez l’enfant français avant l’enseignement formel de la lecture-écriture [Knowledge of letters and writing of the first name in French children who have not yet received formal instruction in reading and writing]. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 45, 111–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ellefson, M., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2009). Learning to label letters by sounds or names: A comparison of England and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 323–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2005). What children are looking at during shared storybook reading: Evidence from eye movement monitoring. Psychological Science, 16, 913–920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Evans, M. A., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 959–989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2006). Changes in letter sound knowledge are associated with development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 143–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Iversen, I., Silberberg, N. E., & Silberberg, M. C. (1970). Sex differences in knowledge of letter and number names in kindergarten. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 31, 79–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Justice, L. M., Pence, K., Bowles, R. B., & Wiggins, A. (2006). An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 374–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Justice, L. M., Skibbe, L., Canning, A., & Lankford, C. (2005). Pre-schoolers, print and storybooks: An observational study using eye movement analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 229–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kim, Y.-S., Petscher, Y., Foorman, B. R., & Zhou, C. (2010). The contributions of phonological awareness and letter-name knowledge to letter-sound acquisition—A cross-classified multilevel model approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 313–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lavine, L. O. (1977). Differentiation of letterlike forms in prereading children. Developmental Psychology, 13, 89–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Levin, I., & Aram, D. (2004). Children’s names contribute to early literacy: A linguistic and a social perspective. In D. Ravid & H. Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development (pp. 219–239). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  15. Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 139–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Levin, I., Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 413–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Locker, L., Jr., Hoffman, L., & Bovaird, J. A. (2007). On the use of multilevel modeling as an alternative to items analysis in psycholinguistic research. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 723–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABC’s of the ABC’s: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.Google Scholar
  19. Piasta, S., & Wagner, R. (2010). Learning letter names and sounds: Effects of instruction, letter type, and phonological processing skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 324–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ravid, D. (2006). Hebrew orthography and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 339–364). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Robins, S., & Treiman, R. (2009). Talking about writing: What we can learn from conversations between parents and their young children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 463–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
  24. Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What’s in a name: Children’s knowledge about the letters in their own names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 105–135). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children’s knowledge and use of upper- and lowercase letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (in press). Similarities among the shapes of writing and their effects on learning. Written Language and Literacy.Google Scholar
  28. Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Pollo, T. C. (2006). Learning about the letter name subset of the vocabulary: Evidence from U.S. and Brazilian preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 211–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Treiman, R., Levin, I., & Kessler, B. (2007). Learning of letter names follows similar principles across languages: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 87–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Treiman, R., Pennington, B. F., Shriberg, L. D., & Boada, R. (2008). Which children benefit from letter names in learning letter sounds? Cognition, 106, 1322–1338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.Google Scholar
  32. Turnbull, K. L. P., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., Justice, L. M., & Wiggins, A. K. (in press). Theoretical explanations for preschoolers’ lowercase alphabet knowledge. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. Google Scholar
  33. Wallentin, M. (2009). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language, 108, 175–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentWashington University in St. LouisSt. LouisUSA
  2. 2.School of EducationTel Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael

Personalised recommendations