Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 24, Issue 5, pp 567–589 | Cite as

From scribbles to scrabble: preschool children’s developing knowledge of written language

  • Cynthia S. Puranik
  • Christopher J. Lonigan
Article

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to concurrently examine the development of written language across different writing tasks and to investigate how writing features develop in preschool children. Emergent written language knowledge of 372 preschoolers was assessed using numerous writing tasks. The findings from this study indicate that children demonstrate knowledge about writing before beginning school and receiving formal instruction. There was clear evidence to support the claim that universal writing features develop before language-specific features. Children as young as 3 years possess knowledge regarding universal and language-specific writing features. Preschoolers appear to progress along a continuum from scribbling to conventional spelling. Although this progression is sequential, children’s writing proficiency is task dependent. Implications of these findings on writing development are discussed.

Keywords

Emergent literacy Emergent writing Language development Preschool Writing Written language 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported, in part, by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD052120) and the Institute of Education Science, US Department of Education (R305A080488). Portions of this work were completed while the first author was supported by a postdoctoral training grant (R305B050032) from the Institute of Education Science, US Department of Education. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and have not been reviewed or approved by the granting agencies. The authors would like to thank Marcy Wyatt and the members of the Preschool Research Group's Assessment Team at the Florida Center for Reading Research for their assistance with data collection, and Stephanie Cute and Lindsay Keffer at the University of Pittsburgh for their assistance with data entry, and scoring.

References

  1. Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 588–610.Google Scholar
  2. Berninger, V. W. (2000). Development of language by hand and its connections to language by ear, mouth, and eye. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 65–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berninger, V. W., Mizokawa, D., Bragg, R., Cartwright, A., & Yates, C. (1994). Intraindividual differences in levels of written language. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 10, 259–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bloodgood, J. (1999). What’s in a name? Children’s name writing and literacy acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 342–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Both-de Vries, A., & Bus, A. G. (2008). Name writing: A first step to phonetic writing? Literacy teaching and learning, 12, 35–55.Google Scholar
  6. Both-de Vries, A., & Bus, A. G. (2009). The proper name as starting point for basic reading skills. Reading and Writing (in press). doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9158-2.
  7. Bus, A. G., Both-de Vries, A. C., de Jong, M. T., Sulzby, E., de Jong, W., & de Jong, E. (2001). Conceptualizations underlying emergent readers’ story writing. Anna Arbor, MI: Ciera Report #2.015.Google Scholar
  8. Bus, A. & Out, D. (2008). Unravelling genetic and environmental components of early literacy: A twin study. Reading and Writing. doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9115-0.
  9. Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  10. Fox, B., & Saracho, O. (1990). Emergent writing: Young children solving the written language puzzle. Early Child Development and Care, 56, 81–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gibson, E., & Levin, H. (1975). The psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  12. Gillanders, C., & Jimenez, R. T. (2004). Reaching for success: A close-up of Mexican immigrant parents in the USA who foster literacy success for their kindergarten children. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 4, 243–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gombert, J. E., & Fayol, M. (1992). Writing in preliterate children. Learning and Instruction, 2, 23–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Goodman, K. S., & Goodman, Y. M. (1979). Learning to read is natural. In L. B. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  15. Graham, S. (1997). Executive control in the revising of students with learning and writing difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 223–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Guttman, L. (1950). The basis for scalogram analysis. In S. A. Stouffer (Ed.), Measurement & prediction (Vol. IV). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  17. Hiebert, E. (1981). Developmental patterns and interrelationships of preschool children’s print awareness. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 236–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hildreth, G. (1936). Developmental sequences in name writing. Child Development, 7, 291–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Justice, L., Pence, K., Bowles, R., & Wiggins, A. (2006b). An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 374–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Levin, I., Both-De Vries, A., Aram, D., & Bus, A. G. (2005). Writing starts with own name writing: From scribbling to conventional spelling in Israeli and Dutch children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 463–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Levin, I., & Bus, A. G. (2003). How is emergent writing based on drawing? Analyses of children’s products and their sorting by children and mothers. Developmental Psychology, 39, 891–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lieberman, E. (1985). Name writing and the preschool child. Phoenix, AZ: Early Childhood Center, Phoenix College. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED269759).Google Scholar
  23. Lomax, R. G., & McGee, L. M. (1987). Young children’s concepts about print and reading: Toward a model of word reading acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 237–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lonigan, C. J., Schatschneider, C., & Westberg, L. (2008). Identification of children’s skills and abilities linked to later outcomes in reading, writing, and spelling. In Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel (pp. 55–106). Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.Google Scholar
  25. Luria, A. R. (1978). The development of writing in a child. In M. Cole (Ed.), The selected writings of A. R. Luria (pp. 146–194). New York: M.E. Sharpe. (Original work published 1929).Google Scholar
  26. Mason, J. M., & Stewart, J. P. (1990). Emergent literacy assessment for instructional use in kindergarten. In L. M. Morrow & J. K. Smith (Eds.), Assessment for instruction in early literacy (pp. 155–175). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  27. McCutchen, D. (1988). “Functional automaticity” in children’s writing: A problem of metacognitive control. Written Communication, 5(3), 306–324.Google Scholar
  28. Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1993). Access to print for children of poverty: Differential effects of adult mediation and literacy-enriched play settings on environmental and functional print tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 95–122.Google Scholar
  29. Phillips, B., Lonigan, C., & Graham, L. (July, 2006). Now I know my ABC’s: Alphabetic order and first name predict letter knowledge development in young children. Interactive paper presented at the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Vancouver, BC.Google Scholar
  30. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Siegler, R. S. (1999). Learning to spell: Variability, choice, and change in children’s strategy use. Child Development, 70, 332–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Saracho, O. (1990). Developmental sequences in three-year-old children’s writing. Early Child Development and Care, 56, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Scott, C. (2005). Learning to write. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 224–258). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  33. Shatil, E., Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten writing to grade 1 literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emergent minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Singer, B., & Bashir, A. (2004). Developmental variations in writing composition skills. In C. A. Stone, E. R. Silliman, B. J. Ehren, & K. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy development and disorders. NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  36. Smith, S. (1976). Learning to read by reading. Language Arts, 53, 297–299.Google Scholar
  37. Sulzby, E. (1989). Assessment of writing and of children’s language while writing. In L. Morrow & J. Smith (Eds.), The role of assessment and measurement in early literacy instruction (pp. 83–109). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hal.Google Scholar
  38. Sulzby, E., Barnhart, J., & Hieshima, J. (1989). Forms of writing and rereading from writing: A preliminary report. In J. Mason (Ed.), Reading and writing connections (pp. 31–63). Needham Hieghts, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  39. Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 727–758). New York: Longman.Google Scholar
  40. Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  41. Tolchinsky-Landsmann, L., & Levin, I. (1985). Writing in preschoolers: An age-related analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 319–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tolchinsky-Landsmann, L., & Levin, I. (1987). Writing in four-to-six-year olds: Representation of semantic and phonetic similarities and differences. Journal of Child Language, 14, 127–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard.Google Scholar
  44. Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What’s in a name: Children’s knowledge about the letters in their own name. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Treiman, R., Cohen, J., Mulqueeny, K., Kessler, B., & Schechtman, S. (2007). Young children’s knowledge about printed names. Child Development, 78, 1458–1471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Villaume, S., & Wilson, L. (1989). Preschool children’s exploration of letters in their own names. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 283–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.Google Scholar
  48. Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickensen (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11–29). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Communication Science and DisordersUniversity of PittsburghPittsburghUSA
  2. 2.Florida Center for Reading ResearchFlorida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA

Personalised recommendations