Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 24, Issue 1, pp 27–53 | Cite as

Letter-name letter-sound and phonological awareness: evidence from Greek-speaking kindergarten children

  • George Manolitsis
  • Eufimia Tafa
Article

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to examine longitudinally the development of letter-sound and letter-name knowledge and their relation to each other and to various aspects of phonological awareness in a sample of Greek kindergarten children who did not know how to read. One hundred twenty children aged 58–69 months were assessed on letter-sound and letter-name knowledge, as well as on phonological awareness skills at the beginning, the middle and the end of kindergarten. The findings indicated that: (a) kindergarten children knew more letter-sounds than letter-names in almost every case across the assessment points; (b) letter-sound knowledge predicted letter-name knowledge slightly better than vice versa; (c) phonological awareness was associated directly with later letter-sound and letter-name knowledge, and (d) the bidirectional hypothesis between phonological awareness and letters knowledge was not confirmed.

Keywords

Letter-sound knowledge Letter-name knowledge Phonological awareness Emergent literacy Kindergarten 

References

  1. Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Research and Evaluation, 11, 253–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
  3. Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blaiklock, K. E. (2004). The importance of letter knowledge in the relationship between phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 36–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blair, R., & Savage, R. (2006). Name writing but not environmental print recognition is related to letter-sound knowledge and phonological awareness in pre-readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 991–1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters–letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing, 18, 211–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowey, J. A. (1994). Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and nonreaders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 134–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read–a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bus, A. G., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Byrne, B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  12. Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 313–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross–linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Chech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cardoso-Martins, C., Resende, S. M., & Rodrigues, L. A. (2002). Letter name knowledge and the ability to learn to read by processing letter-phoneme relations in words: Evidence from Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 409–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cross Thematic Curriculum Framework for Kindergarten. (2003). Ministerial Decision G2/21072 Government Gazette, 304.Google Scholar
  19. de Jong, P. F. (2007). Phonological awareness and the use of similarity in letter-sound learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 131–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. de Jong, P. F., & Olson, R. K. (2004). Early predictors of letter knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 254–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dunn-Rankin, P. (1968). The similarity of lower-case letters of the English alphabet. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 990–995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ehri, L. C. (1984). How orthography alters spoken language competencies in children learning to read and spell. In J. Downing & R. Valtin (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
  23. Ellefson, M. R., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2009). Learning to label letters by sounds or names: A comparison of England and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 323–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Evans, M. A., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 959–989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75.Google Scholar
  26. Foulin, J. N. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 129–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2006). Changes in letter sound knowledge are associated with development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 143–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Fries, C. C. (1962). Linguistics and reading. New York: Holt, Rhinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  29. Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency in English and Greek: A cross-linguistic comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gibson, E. J., Osser, H., & Pick, A. D. (1963). A study of the development of grapheme-phoneme correspondences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 142–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2000). Two alternative ways to model the relation between reading accuracy and phonological awareness at preschool age. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hulme, C., Caravolas, M., Malkova, G., & Brigstocke, S. (2005a). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. Cognition, 97, B1–B11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hulme, C., Snowling, M., Caravolas, M., & Caroll, J. (2005b). Phonological skills are (probably) one cause of success in learning to read: A comment on castles and coltheart. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 351–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Johnston, R. S., Anderson, M., & Holligan, C. (1996). Knowledge of the alphabet and explicit awareness of phonemes in pre-readers: The nature of the relationship. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 217–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Joreskog, K., & Sorbom, D. (1993). Lisrel 8: Structural equation modelling with SIMPLIS command language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associated.Google Scholar
  37. Kim, Y.-S. (in press). The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: The relative contribution of letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness and their interrelationship in Korean. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9131-0.
  38. Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word recognition in Grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 367–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Levin, I., Shantil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 139–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fisher, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Manolitsis, G. (2000). Metrisi ke aksiologisi metaglosikon ikanotiton pedion ilikias 5–6 eton. [Measurement and evaluation of 5–6 years old children’s metalinguistic abilities]. Athens, Greece: Grigoris.Google Scholar
  45. Manolitsis, G. (2004). Gnosi grammaton ke fonologiki epignosi: Mia erevna sysxetisis se pedia nipiagogiou [Letter knowledge and phonological awareness: A correlational study in kindergarten children]. In P. Papoulia-Tzelepi & E. Tafa (Eds.), Glossa kai grammatismos sti nea xilietia. [Language and literacy in the new millennium] (pp. 53–67). Athens, Greece: Typothito.Google Scholar
  46. Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., & Parrila, R. (2008). Cognitive components and reading development: From kindergarten to second grade. Paper presented at the 1st Pan-Hellenic Conference on Developmental Psychology, Athens, Greece.Google Scholar
  47. Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., Stephenson, K., & Parrila, R. (in press). Beginning to read across languages varying in orthographic consistency: Comparing the effects of non-cognitive and cognitive predictors. Learning and Instruction. doi: 10.1016/jlearninstruc.2008.07.003.
  48. McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.Google Scholar
  49. McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. (2002). Cross–cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 1392–1407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. McCormick, C. E., & Mason, J. M. (1986). Intervention procedures for increasing preschool children’s interest in and knowledge about reading. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  51. Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mouzaki, A., Protopapas, A., & Tsantoula, D. (2008). Proscholiki diktes prognosis anagnostikon deksiotiton stin A dimotikou [Preschool indices predicting the development of reading skills in 1st grade]. Epistimes tis Agogis [Education Sciences] 1, 71–88.Google Scholar
  53. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 3–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Naslund, J. C., & Schneider, W. (1996). Kindergarten letter knowledge, phonological skills, and memory processes: Relative effects on early literacy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 30–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Nespor, M. (1996). Fonologia [Phonology]. Athens, Greece: Patakis.Google Scholar
  57. Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G., & Kendeou, P. (2009). The dimensionality of phonological abilities in Greek. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 127–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Patel, T., Snowling, M., & de Jong, P. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and in Dutch. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 785–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Porpodas, C. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 404–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Program of Studies of the Modern Greek Language in Pre-Primary Education—Kindergarten and Primary School. (2003). Ministerial Decision G1/58, Government Gazette, 93/v. Β.Google Scholar
  61. Protopapas, A., & Vlahou, E. (in press). A comparative quantitative analysis of Greek orthographic transparency. Behavior Research Methods.Google Scholar
  62. Raven, J. C. (1956). Coloured progressive matrices: Sets A, AB, B. London: Lewis.Google Scholar
  63. Read, C., Zhang, Y.-F., Nie, H.-Y., & Ding, B.-Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 31–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Schneider, W., Roth, E., & Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 284–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in Kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 59–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309–1324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Smythe, P. C., Stennett, R. G., Hardy, M., & Wilson, H. R. (1971). Developmental patterns in elemental skills: Knowledge of upper-case and lower-case letter names. Journal of Reading Behavior, 3, 24–33.Google Scholar
  69. Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Stuart, M., & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Harper Collins College Publishers.Google Scholar
  72. Tafa, E. (2003). Ti gnorizoun ta pedia tis proscholikis ilikias gia ta grammata tou alphavitou ke pos ta chrisimopioun gia na grapsoun to onoma tous [What kindergarten children know about the alphabet letters and how they use them to write their own names]. In Department of Preschool Education (Ed.), Proceedings of the international conference for literacy and mathematics in preschool education (pp. 293–307). Rethymnon, Greece: University of Crete.Google Scholar
  73. Tafa, E. (2004). Literacy activities in half-and whole day Greek kindergarten classrooms. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2, 85–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tafa, E., & Manolitsis, G. (2008). A longitudinal literacy profile of Greek precocious readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 43, 165–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Tombaidis, D. (1995). Didaskalia neoellinikis glossas [Teaching Modern Greek]. Thessaloniki, Greece: Vanias.Google Scholar
  76. Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1128–1142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Tolvanen, A., Leskinen, E., Leppänen, P. H. T., et al. (2007). Modeling the early paths of phonological awareness and factors supporting its development in children with and without familial risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 73–103.Google Scholar
  78. Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What’s in a name: Children’s knowledge about the letters in their own names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children’s knowledge and use of upper-and lowercase letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. (1998). The foundation of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.Google Scholar
  81. Triantafilidis, M. (1975). Mikri neoelliniki grammatiki [Small modern Greek grammar] (2nd ed.). Thessaloniki, Greece: Institute of Modern Greek Studies.Google Scholar
  82. Tsakris, P. (1970). I efiia ton ellinopedon tis scholikis ilikias (5–12 eton) [The intelligence of school-age Greek children (5–12 years old)]. Athens, Greece: Author.Google Scholar
  83. Tsantoula, D., Protopapas, A., & Mouzaki, A. (2004). Proscholiki deiktes anagnostikon deksiotiton [Indices of literacy readiness at preschool age]. In M. Tzouriadou (Ed.), I Evropaiki diastasi tis idikis agogis [The European Dimension of Special Education] (pp. 208–217). Thessaloniki, Greece: University Studio Press.Google Scholar
  84. Tunmer, W. E., Herriman, M. L., & Nesdale, A. R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 134–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashote, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Wallach, L., Wallach, M. A., Dozier, M. G., & Kaplan, N. E. (1977). Poor children learning to read do not have trouble with auditory discrimination but do have trouble with phoneme recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 36–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Winskel, H., & Widjaja, V. (2007). Phonological awareness, letter knowledge, and literacy development in Indonesian beginner readers and spellers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 23–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 413–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Preschool EducationUniversity of CreteRethymno, CreteGreece

Personalised recommendations