Reading and Writing

, Volume 23, Issue 7, pp 835–851 | Cite as

Looking at the keyboard or the monitor: relationship with text production processes

  • Roger Johansson
  • Åsa Wengelin
  • Victoria Johansson
  • Kenneth Holmqvist
Article

Abstract

In this paper we explored text production differences in an expository text production task between writers who looked mainly at the keyboard and writers who looked mainly at the monitor. Eye-tracking technology and keystroke-logging were combined to systematically describe and define these two groups in respect of the complex interplay between text production and the reading of one’s own emerging text. Findings showed that monitor gazers typed significantly faster and were more productive writers. They also read their own text more, and they frequently read in parallel with writing. Analysis of fixation durations suggests that more cognitive processing is in use during reading in parallel with writing than during reading in pauses. Keyboard gazers used the left and right cursor keys significantly more. We suggest that this is because they revised their texts in a much more serial mode than monitor gazers. Finally, analysis of the characteristics of the final texts showed no differences between the groups.

Keywords

Reading during writing Typing Visual attention Keystroke-logging Eye-tracking 

References

  1. Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In M. Torrance, D. Galbraith, & L. van. Waes (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (Studies in Writing, Vol. 20, pp. 13–29). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  2. Alvès, R. A., Castro, S. L., de Sousa, L., & Strömqvist, S. (2007). Influence of typing skill on pause-execution cycles in written composition. In M. Torrance, D. Galbraith, & L. van. Waes (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (Studies in Writing, Vol. 20, pp. 55–65). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  3. Andersson, B., Dahl, J., Holmqvist, K., Holsanova, J., Johansson, V., Karlsson, H., Strömqvist, S., Tufvesson, S., & Wengelin, Å. (2006). Combining keystroke logging with eye tracking. In L. Van Waes, M. Leijten, & C. Neuwirth (Eds.), Writing and digital media (Studies in Writing, Vol. 17. pp. 166–172). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Beers, S. F., Quinlan, T., & Harbaugh, A. G. (2009). Reading during writing and adolescent students’ writing competence: An eyetracking study. Reading and writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal (this issue).Google Scholar
  6. Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Berman, R. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Cross-linguistic perspectives on the development of text-production abilities: Speech and writing. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bourdin, B., & Fayol, M. (1994). Is written language production more difficult than oral language production—a working-memory approach. International Journal of Psychology, 29(5), 591–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fayol, M. (1999). From on-line management problems to strategies in written composition. In M. Torrance & G. Jeffery (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing (Studies in writing, Vol. 3, pp. 13–24). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Gentner, D. R., Larochelle, S., & Grudin, J. (1988). Lexical, sublexical, and peripheral effects in skilled typewriting. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 524–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Grabowski, J. (2008). The internal structure of university students’ keyboard skills. Journal of writing research, 1(1), 27–52.Google Scholar
  12. Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. (1980). Identifying the organisation of the writing process. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Herrström, M. (1998). Djur lästest. Simrishamn: Ordfabriken.Google Scholar
  14. Holmqvist, K., Johansson, V., Strömqvist, S., & Wengelin, Å. (2002). Studying reading and writing online. In S. Strömqvist (Ed.), The diversity of languages and language learning (pp. 103–123). Lund, Sweden: Lund University, Centre for Languages and Literature.Google Scholar
  15. Hunt, K. (1970). Recent measures in syntactic development. In M. Lester (Ed.), Readings in applied transformation grammar (pp. 187–200). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  16. Inhoff, A. W., & Gordon, A. (1998). Eye movements and eye-hand coordination during typing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(6), 153–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Inhoff, A. W., & Wang, J. W. (1992). The encoding of text, manual movement planning, and eye-hand coordination during copytyping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 437–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Johansson, M.-G. (1992). LS klassdiagnoser i läsning och skrivning för högstadiet och gymnasiet. Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiförlaget.Google Scholar
  19. Johansson, R., Johansson, V., Wengelin, Å., & Holmqvist, K. (2008). Reading during writing: Four groups of writers. Working Papers 53 (pp. 43–59). Lund, Sweden: Lund University, Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar
  20. Kaufer, D. S., Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. (1986). Composing written sentences. Research in the Teaching of English, 20(2), 121–140.Google Scholar
  21. Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 57–71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Kellogg, R. T. (2001). Competition for working memory among writing processes. American Journal of Psychology, 114(2), 175–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kollmorgen, S., & Holmqvist, K. (2007). Automatically detecting reading in eye tracking data. Lund, Sweden: Lund University, Department of Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
  24. Liversedge, S. P., & Findlay, J. M. (2000). Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 6–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Logan, F. A. (1999). Errors in copy typewriting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1760–1773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Logan, G. D. (2003). Simon-type effects: Chronometric evidence for keypress schemata in typewriting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 741–757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McCutchen, D., Covill, A., Hoyne, S. H., & Mildes, K. (1994). Individual differences in writing skill: Implications of translating fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 256–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. McKee, G., Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2000). Measuring vocabulary diversity using dedicated software. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 15, 323–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Optimal viewing position in words and the strategy-tactics theory of eye movements in reading. In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 333–354). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  30. Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2002). Concurrent activation of high- and low-level production processes in written composition. Memory and Cognition, 30(4), 594–600.Google Scholar
  31. Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2002). Suppressing visual feedback in written composition: Effects on processing demands and coordination of the writing processes. International Journal of Psychology, 37(4), 209–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ransdell, S., & Levy, C. M. (1996). Working memory constraints on writing quality and fluency. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 93–105). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105, 125–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6, 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Starr, M. S., & Rayner, K. (2001). Eye movements during reading: Some current controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4), 156–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Strömqvist, S., & Karlsson, H. (2001). ScriptLog for Windowsuser’s manual. Technical Report. Lund University: Department of Linguistics; and University College of Stavanger: Centre for Reading Research.Google Scholar
  38. Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., & Quinlan, T. (2009). Reading during sentence composing and error correction: A multilevel analysis of the influences of task complexity. Reading and writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal (this issue).Google Scholar
  39. Wengelin, Å. (2006). Examining Pauses in Writing: Theory, Methods and Empirical Data. In K. Sullivan & E. Lindgren (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging and writing (Studies in Writing, Vol. 18, pp. 107–130). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  40. Wengelin, Å. (2007). The word level focus in text production by adults with reading and writing difficulties. In M. Torrance, L. Van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition research and applications (Studies in Writing, Vol. 20, pp. 67–82). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  41. Wengelin, Å., Torrance, M., Holmqvist, K., Simpson, S., Galbraith, D., Johansson, V., & Johansson, R. (2009). Combined eye-tracking and keystroke-logging methods for studying cognitive processes in text production. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 337–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Roger Johansson
    • 1
  • Åsa Wengelin
    • 1
  • Victoria Johansson
    • 1
  • Kenneth Holmqvist
    • 2
  1. 1.Centre for Languages and LiteratureLund UniversityLundSweden
  2. 2.Humanities LaboratoryLund UniversityLundSweden

Personalised recommendations