Reading and Writing

, Volume 21, Issue 3, pp 231–253

Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition

  • Chen-Huei Liao
  • George K. Georgiou
  • Rauno Parrila
Article

Abstract

We examined the relationship between rapid naming speed (RAN) and Chinese character recognition accuracy and fluency. Sixty-three grade 2 and 54 grade 4 Taiwanese children were administered four RAN tasks (colors, digits, Zhu-Yin-Fu-Hao, characters), and two character recognition tasks. RAN tasks accounted for more reading variance in grade 4 than in grade 2, and graphological RAN tasks accounted for more reading variance than RAN Colors. After controlling for age, nonverbal intelligence, phonological sensitivity, short-term memory, and orthographic processing, RAN tasks were significant predictors of character recognition fluency in grade 2, and of both accuracy and fluency in grade 4.

Keywords

Character recognition Chinese reading Rapid naming 

References

  1. Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disability over time. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bowers, P. G., Golden, J. O., Kennedy, A., & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 173–218). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  4. Bowers, P. G., & Newby-Clarke, E. (2002). The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 109–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowers, P. G., & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Caravolas, M. (2006). The foundations of literacy in Czech: Not so different from English after all? Paper presented at the 11th annual conference of the Society for the Scientific Studies of Reading. Canada: Vancouver.Google Scholar
  9. Cardoso-Martins, C., & Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 27–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chan, L., & Wang, L. (2003). Linguistic awareness in learning to read Chinese: A comparative study of Beijing and Hong Kong children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 91–106). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  11. Chen, M. J. (2003). Component skills for reading Chinese in primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 19–33). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  12. Cheung, H., McBride-Chang, C., & Chow, B. W.-Y. (2006). Reading Chinese. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 421–438). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Chow, B. W.-Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Burgess, S. (2005). Phonological processing skills and early reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese kindergarteners learning to read English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 81–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Empirisoft Corporation (2006). DirectRT Research Software (v2004). New York: Empirisoft.Google Scholar
  18. Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 199–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Glass, G. V., & Hopkins, K. D. (1984). Statistical methods in education and psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  21. Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997a). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997b). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946–951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Lee, S.-H., Tsang, S.-M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2000). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific learning Difficulties Research Team.Google Scholar
  25. Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Ho, C. S.-H., & Lai, D. N.-C. (1999). Naming-speed and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 173–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ho, C. S.-H., Ng, T.-T., & Ng, W.-K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 849–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ho, C. S.-H., Yau, P. W.-Y., & Au, A. (2003). Development of orthographic knowledge and its relationship with reading and spelling among Chinese kindergarten and primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading development (pp. 51–71). New Haven, CT: Greenwood.Google Scholar
  29. Hoosain, R. (1991). Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  30. Hu, C.-F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 73–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Huang, H., & Hanley, J. (1997). A longitudinal study of phonological awareness, visual skills, and Chinese reading acquisition among first-graders in Taiwan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 249–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Huang, H.-S. (2001). Graded Chinese character recognition test. Taipei, Taiwan: Psychology Publication.Google Scholar
  34. Ju, D., & Jackson, N. E. (1995). Graphic and phonological processing in Chinese character identification. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 299–313.Google Scholar
  35. Kang, C. (2004). Phonological awareness and naming speed in good and poor Chinese readers. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Hong Kong, China.Google Scholar
  36. Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B. K., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 51–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Levelt, W., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lu, T.-H. (2003). The relationship of phonological processing (phonological awareness, verbal short-term memory, and rapid naming) and visual short-term memory to reading disabilities in Chinese children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, Iowa City.Google Scholar
  40. Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Mayringer, H., Wimmer, W., & Landerl, K. (1998). Phonological skills and literacy acquisition in German. In P. Reitsma, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 147–161). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  44. McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W.-Y., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 99–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 50–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. V. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 1392–1407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McBride-Chang, C., & Zhong, Y. (2003). A longitudinal study of effects of phonological processing, visual skills, and speed of processing on Chinese character acquisition among Hong Kong Chinese kindergartners. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 37–49). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  49. Müller, K., & Brady, S. (2001). Correlates of early reading performance in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 757–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Naglieri, J. A., & Das, J. P. (1997). Das-Naglieri cognitive assessment system. Itasca, IL: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  51. Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatized naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pak, A., Cheng-Lai, A., Tso, I., Shu, H., Li, W., & Anderson, R. (2005). Visual chunking skills of Hong Kong Children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 437–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & McQuarrie, L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development? Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 3–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Patel, T., Snowling, M. J., & de Jong, P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 785–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Pedhazur, E. G. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed.). New York: CBS College Publishing.Google Scholar
  57. Savage, R., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain and Language, 93, 152–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 115–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Shen, H. H., & Bear, D. R. (2000). Development of orthographic skills in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 197–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Shu, H. (2003). Chinese writing system and learning to read. International Journal of Psychology, 38, 274–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 78–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1999). Learning to read Chinese: The development of metalinguistic awareness. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 1–18). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  64. Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading. Developmental Psychology, 37, 886–899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  66. Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 102, 8781–8785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Theios, J., & Amrhein, P. C. (1989). Theoretical analysis of the cognitive processing of lexical and pictorial stimuli: Reading, naming, and visual and conceptual comparisons. Psychological Review, 96, 5–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Uhry, J. K. (2002). Kindergarten phonological awareness and rapid serial naming as predictors of Grade 2 reading and spelling. In E. Witruk, A. D. Friederici & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Basic functions of language, reading, and reading disability (pp. 299–313). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  71. van den Bos, K., Zijlstra, B. J., & lutje-Spelberg, H. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors, and pictured objects, and word-reading speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 25–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C. A, Hecht, S., Barker, T., Burgess, T., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Wang, Q. (2005). Predicting Chinese children’s reading development from kindergarten to second grade. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA.Google Scholar
  75. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Yeh, S.-L., & Li, J.-L. (2002). Role of structure and component in judgments of visual similarity of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 933–947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Young, A., & Bowers, P. (1995). Individual differences and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 428–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Chen-Huei Liao
    • 1
  • George K. Georgiou
    • 1
  • Rauno Parrila
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Educational PsychologyUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada

Personalised recommendations