Reading and Writing

, Volume 20, Issue 8, pp 753–784

Reading in two orthographies: A cross-linguistic study of Dutch average and poor readers who learn English as a second language

  • Eleni Morfidi
  • Aryan van der Leij
  • Peter F. de Jong
  • Femke Scheltinga
  • Judith Bekebrede
Article

Abstract

The present study investigated the reading of secondary school students in their first and second language (L1, L2). Twenty-six average and twenty-six poor readers matched on age, gender, listening and reading comprehension participated. They were native Dutch speakers who started learning English at secondary school (grade 7). We examined whether differences in L2 between the two groups reflect differences in L1 with regard to reading and relevant subskills. In addition, the relationship between reading and its predictors within and across the two languages was investigated. Between group differences were similar in L1 and L2 when task conditions involved high levels of phonological and orthographic complexity or demanded speeded processing. Furthermore, serial rapid naming predicted speeded word reading in both languages and L2 text reading accuracy, while L2 phoneme awareness and orthographic knowledge explained unique variance in L2 text reading accuracy. Cross-linguistic prediction revealed that speeded word reading predicted its counterpart from L1 to L2 and vice versa. Serial rapid naming explained additional variance in the prediction of L2 from L1. After exclusion of the reading predictor from the model, serial rapid naming was the most consistent cross-linguistic predictor, while L2 orthographic knowledge explained a small amount of unique variance in L1 speeded word reading.

Keywords

Poor readers Phonological skills Orthography Second language 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Badian N. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction Annals of Dyslexia 51: 179–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bowey J. A., McGuigan M., Ruschena A. (2005). On the associations between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account Journal of Research in Reading 28: 400–422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bruck M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits Developmental Psychology 28(5): 874–885CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brus B. T., Voeten M. J. M. (1973). Een-minuut-test [One-minute-test] Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Berkhout B. VGoogle Scholar
  5. Caravolas M., Volin J., Hulme C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 92: 107–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cardoso-Martins C., Pennington B. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability Scientific Studies of Reading 8: 28–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cisero C., Royer J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness Contemporary Educational Psychology 20: 275–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis of the behavioral science (2nd ed) New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  9. Comeau L., Cormier P., Grandmaison E., Lacroix D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language Journal of Educational Psychology 9(1): 29–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Compton D. (2000). Modeling the growth of decoding skills in first grade children Scientific Studies of Reading 4: 219–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cossu G. (1999). Biological constraints on literacy acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 11: 213–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cunningham A., Perry K., Stanovich K. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 14: 549–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cunningham A., Perry K., Stanovich K., Share D. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 82: 185–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cunningham A., Stanovich K. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience Journal of Educational Psychology 84(4): 733–740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. de Jong P. F., van der Leij A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study Journal of Educational Psychology 91(3): 450–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. de Jong P. F., van der Leij A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography Journal of Educational Psychology 95(1): 22–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Denckla M. B., Rudel R. G. (1974). ‘Rapid automatised naming’ of pictured objects, colours, letters and numbers by normal children Cortex 10: 186–202Google Scholar
  18. Dunn L. M., Dunn L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance ServiceGoogle Scholar
  19. Durgunoglu A. Y., Nagy W., Hancin-Bhatt B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness Journal of Educational Psychology 85(3): 543–565CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Elbro C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 1–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Elbro C., Borstrom I., Petersen D. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctiveness of phonological representations of lexical items Reading Research Quarterly 33(1): 36–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Elbro C., Nielsen I., Petersen D. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items Annals of Dyslexia 44: 205–226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Elliott C. D. (1983). British Ability Scales, manual 2, technical handbook Windsor: NFER-NelsonGoogle Scholar
  24. Fawcett A. J., Nicolson R. I. (1996). The Dyslexia Screening Test London: The Psychological CorporationGoogle Scholar
  25. Felton R., Brown I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 39–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Foorman B. R., Francis D. J. (1994). Exploring connections among reading, spelling, and phonemic segmentation during first grade Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6: 65–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Foorman B. F., Francis D. J., Fletcher J. M., Lynn A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: Comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level-match designs Journal of Educational Psychology 88(4): 639–652CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Fowler A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In: Brady S. A., Shankweiler D. P. (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 97–117)Google Scholar
  29. Gallagher A. M., Laxon V., Armstrong E., Frith U. (1996). Phonological difficulties in high functioning dyslexics Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 499–509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ganschow L., Sparks R., Javorsky J., Pohlman J., Bishop-Marbury A. (1991). Identifying native language difficulties among foreign language learners in college: A ‘foreign’ language learning disability? Journal of Learning Disabilities 24(9): 530–541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Geva E., Siegel L. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages Reading and Writing 12: 1–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Goswami, U. (1999). Towards a theoretical framework for understanding reading development and dyslexia in different orthographies. In I. Lundberg, F. E. Tonnesen, & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic PublishersGoogle Scholar
  33. Gottardo A., Yan B., Siegel L. S., Wade-Wooley L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing Journal of Educational Psychology 93(3): 530–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hacquebord I. (1992). ‘Alle leerlingen’. Toets voor begrijpend lezen. [‘All students’. Test for reading comprehension] Groningen: Universiteit van GroningenGoogle Scholar
  35. Harris M., Hatano G. (1999). Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  36. Ho C. S., Lai D. N. (2000). Naming speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia Learning and Individual Differences 11: 173–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kuijpers C., van der Leij A., Been P., van Leeuwen T., ter Keurs M., Schreuder R., van den Bos K. P. (2003). Leesproblemen in het voortgezet onderwijs en de volwassenheid [Reading problems in secondary school and adulthood] Pedagogische Studiën 80(4): 272–287Google Scholar
  38. Landerl K., Wimmer H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children Applied Psycholinguistics 21: 243–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Landerl K., Wimmer H., Frith U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison Cognition 63: 315–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lindsey K. A., Manis F. R., Bailey C. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners Journal of Educational Psychology 95(3): 482–494CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lundberg I., Frost J., Petersen O. P. (1988). Reading and spelling skills the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness in kindergarten Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 21: 159–173Google Scholar
  42. Manis F. R., Custodio R., Szeszulski P. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 56: 64–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Manis F. R., Seidenberg M. S., Doi L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders Scientific Studies of Reading 3: 129–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. McBride-Chang C., Manis F., Seidenberg M., Custodio R., Doi L. (1993). Print exposure as a predictor of word reading and reading comprehension in disabled and nondisabled readers Journal of Educational Psychology 85(2): 230–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Miller-Guron L., Lundberg I. (2000). Dyslexia and second language reading: A second bite at the apple?Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12: 41–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Muter V., Hulme C., Snowling M., Taylor S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming predicts early progress in learning to read Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 65: 370–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Neale M. D., Whetton C., Caspall L., McCulloch K. (1997). Neale Analysis of Reading Ability-Revised. Manual for psychological services Windsor: WhurrGoogle Scholar
  48. Olson R., Forsberg H., Wise B., Rack J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the learning disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing CoGoogle Scholar
  49. Olson R. K., Wise B., Conners F., Rack J., Fulker D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences Journal of Learning Disabilities 22: 339–348Google Scholar
  50. Patel T. K., Snowling M., de Jong P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch Journal of Educational Psychology 96: 785–797CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Perfetti C. A. (1985). Reading ability New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  52. Perfetti C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In: P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp.145–174). City, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  53. Porpodas C. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek Journal of Learning Disabilities 32(5): 406–416Google Scholar
  54. Rack J. P., Snowling M. J., Olson R. K. (1992). The non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review Reading Research Quarterly 27(1): 29–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Reitsma P. (1983). Word-specific knowledge in beginning reading Journal of Research in Reading 6: 41–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Riccio C. A., Amado A., Jimenez S., Hasbrouck J. E., Imhoff B., Denton C. (2001). Cross-linguistic transfer of phonological processing: Development of a measure of phonological processing in Spanish Bilingual Research Journal 25(4): 416–436Google Scholar
  57. Seymour P. (1986). Cognitive analysis of dyslexia London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
  58. Seymour P., Aro M., Erskine J. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies British Journal of Psychology 94: 143–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Share D. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition Cognition 55: 151–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Share D. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72: 95–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Siegel L. S., Share D., Geva E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics Psychological Science 6(4): 250–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Snowling M., Goulandris N., Defty N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 653–669CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Spring C., Davis J. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 315–344Google Scholar
  64. Stanovich K., West R. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing Reading Research Quarterly 24(4): 402–427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Thorndike E. L., Lorge I. (1944). The teacher’s word book of 30000 words New York: Teachers CollegeGoogle Scholar
  66. Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A., Alexander A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationship with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency and the brain (333–355). Parkton, MD: York PressGoogle Scholar
  67. van den Bos K. P. (1996). BELL 96 The Netherlands: Department of Special Education, Groningen UniversityGoogle Scholar
  68. van den Bos K. P.. (1998). IQ, phonological awareness and continuous-naming speed related to Dutch poor decoding children’s performance on two word identification tests Dyslexia 4: 73–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. van den Bos K. P., Spelberg H. C., Scheepstra A. J. M., de Vries J. (1994). De Klepel. Vorm A en B. Een test voor de leesvaardigheid van pseudowoorden. Verantwoording, handleiding, diagnostiek en behandeling. [The Klepel. Form A and B. A test of reading pseudowords]. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: BerkhoutGoogle Scholar
  70. van den Bosch K., van Bon W. H. J., Schreuder R. (1995). Poor readers’ decoding skills: Effects of training with limited exposure duration Reading Research Quarterly 30: 110–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. van den Bos K. P., Zijlstra B. J., Lutje Spelberg H. C. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colours, pictured objects and word-reading speed Scientific Studies of Reading 6: 25–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. van der Leij A., Morfidi E. (2006) Core deficits and variable differences in Dutch poor readers learning English Journal of Learning Disabilities 39: 74–90Google Scholar
  73. van der Leij A., van Daal V. (1999). Automatization aspects of dyslexia: Speed limitation in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demands, and orthographic compensation Journal of Learning Disabilities 32(5): 417–428Google Scholar
  74. Vellutino F. R., Fletcher J. M., Snowling M. J., Scanlon D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 45: 2–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Vellutino F. R., Scanlon D. M. (1986). Linguistic coding and metalinguistic awareness: Their relationship to verbal memory and code acquisition in poor and normal readers. In D. B. Yaden, S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning reading (pp. 115–141). NH: Heinemann PortsmouthGoogle Scholar
  76. Vellutino F. R., Scanlon D. M., Tanzman M. S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing CoGoogle Scholar
  77. Wade-Wooley L., Geva E. (2000). Processing novel phonemic contrasts in acquisition of L2 word reading Scientific Studies of Reading 4(4): 295–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Wagner R. K., Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A.. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study Developmental Psychology 30: 73–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wagner R. K., Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A. Hecht S. A., Barker T. A., Burgess S. R. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled reader: A 5-year longitudinal study Developmental Psychology 33: 468–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Willems M. M., Oud-de Glas M. M. B. (1990). Vocabulaire-selectie voor het vreemde-talenonderwijs [Vocabulary selection for foreign language education] Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Instituut voor Toegepaste Sociale WetenschappenGoogle Scholar
  81. Wimmer H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system Applied Psycholinguistics 14: 1–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Wimmer H. (1996). The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 1–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Wimmer H., Mayringer H., Landerl K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography Journal of Educational Psychology 92(4): 668–680CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Wolf M., Bally H., Morris R. (1986). Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors In: Berninger V. W. (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: KluwerGoogle Scholar
  85. Wolf M., Bowers P. G. (1999). The double deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 415–438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Yap R., van der Leij A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics: An automatic decoding deficit? Reading and Writing 5: 261–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Ziegler J. C., Goswami U. (2005). Reading acquisition developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory Psychological Bulletin 131(1): 3–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eleni Morfidi
    • 1
  • Aryan van der Leij
    • 1
  • Peter F. de Jong
    • 1
  • Femke Scheltinga
    • 1
  • Judith Bekebrede
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Educational SciencesUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations