Reading and Writing

, Volume 20, Issue 3, pp 251–272 | Cite as

Writing through retellings: an exploratory study of language-impaired and dyslexic populations

  • Cynthia S. Puranik
  • Linda J. Lombardino
  • Lori J. Altmann
Article

Abstract

This study was designed to examine differences and similarities in the writing of 15 language-impaired, 17 dyslexic and 15 typically developing control subjects matched on chronological age. Subjects ranging in age from 11 to 21 years were required to produce a written language sample using an expository text-retell procedure. The writing of these groups was compared on eight variables across discourse, T-unit, sentence, and word levels. Control subjects performed better than language-impaired and dyslexic subjects on all writing variables. Dyslexic subjects showed better performance than the language-impaired subjects on several variables including, (a) number of T-units, (b) number of ideas, (c) total number of words, and (4) number of different words while showing comparable performance on percentage of spelling and production of grammatically correct sentences. These findings support Bishop and Snowling’s [Psychol. Bull. 130 (2004) 858] position that the differences between these two clinical populations exist in the non-phonological dimensions of language.

Keywords

Dyslexia Language impairment Retellings SLI Writing Written language disorders 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was partly funded by a University of Florida Alumni Fellowship and a Bamford-Lahey Children’s Foundation Scholarship awarded to the first author. We are grateful to all the subjects for their participation in this project. The authors would like to thank Susan Kemper for her suggestion regarding calculation of ideas. Thanks to Pam Greyson and Nirupama Gupta for their assistance with linguistic analysis, and to Elizabeth Mikell and Lori Lamb for assistance with data collection. Portions of this paper were presented at the 11th annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, held in June 2004 in Amsterdam.

References

  1. Aaron P. G., Joshi R. M. (1992). Reading problems: Consultation and remediation. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Altmann, L., Puranik, C., Mikell, L., & Lombardino, L. (2005, June). Grammatical sentence production in individuals with and without dyslexia. In Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Toronto, Canada Google Scholar
  3. Bar-Shalom E. G., Crain S., Shankweiler D. (1993). A comparison of comprehension and production abilities of good and poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics 14:197–227Google Scholar
  4. Bishop D. V. M. (1982). Comprehension of spoken, written, and signed sentences in childhood language disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 23:1–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bishop D. V. M., Clarkson B. (2003). Written language as a window into residual language deficits: A study of children with persistent and residual speech and language impairments. Cortex 39:215–237Google Scholar
  6. Bishop D. V. M., Snowling M. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin 130:858–886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Byrne B. (1981). Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: Is a weak phonetic memory code responsible? Applied Psycholinguistics 2:201–212Google Scholar
  8. Carrow-Woolfolk E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MNGoogle Scholar
  9. Casalis S., Colé P., Sopo D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 54:114–138Google Scholar
  10. Dockrell J., Lindsay G. (2000). Meeting the needs of children with specific speech and language difficulties. European Journal of Special Needs Education 15:24–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. de Jong P. (1998). Working memory deficits of reading disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 70:75–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ellis Weismer S. (1996). Capacity limitations in working memory: The impact on lexical and morphological learning by children with language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders 17:33–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ellis Weismer S., Evans J., Hesketh L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 4:1249–1260Google Scholar
  14. Fey M., Catts H., Proctor-Williams K., Tomblin B., Zhang X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47:1301–1318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fraser J., & Conti-Ramsden G. (2005, June). Reading and language disorders: two sides of the same coin? In Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Toronto, Canada Google Scholar
  16. Gathercole S., Baddeley A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language 29:336–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gathercole S., Baddeley A. (1993). Working memory and language processing. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hove, U.KGoogle Scholar
  18. Gazella J., Stockman I. (2003). Children’s story retelling under different modality and task conditions: Implications for standardizing language sampling procedures. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12:61–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gillam R., Carlile R. (1997). Oral reading and story retelling of students with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 28:30–41Google Scholar
  20. Gillam R., Johnston J. (1992). Spoken and written language relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35:1303–1315Google Scholar
  21. Harp B. (2000). The handbook of literacy assessment and evaluation 2nd ed.). Christopher-Gordon Publishers, Inc., Norwood, MAGoogle Scholar
  22. Houck C., Billingsley B. (1989). Written expression of students with and without learning disabilities: Differences across the grades. Journal of Learning Disabilities 22:561–565Google Scholar
  23. Hunt, K. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, Research Report No. 3Google Scholar
  24. Joanisse M., Manis F., Keating P., Seidenberg M. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 77:30–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kamhi A.G., Catts H.W. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 51:337–347Google Scholar
  26. Kiewra K., Mayer R., Christensen M., Kim S., Risch N. (1991). Effects of repetition on recall and note-taking strategies for learning form lectures. Journal of Educational Psychology 83:120–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kibby M., Marks W., Morgan S., Long C. (2004). Specific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: a working memory approach. Journal of Learning Disabilities 37:349–362Google Scholar
  28. Lahey M., Bloom L. (1994). Variability and language learning disabilities. In: Wallach G., Butler K. (eds) Language learning disabilities in school-age children and adolescents. Macmillan, New York, pp. 354–372Google Scholar
  29. Leonard L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  30. Leikin M., Assayag-Bouskila O. (2004). Expression of syntactic complexity in sentence comprehension: A comparison between dyslexic and regular readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 17:801–821CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, Research Report No. 18Google Scholar
  32. Mackie C., Dockrell J. (2004). The nature of written language deficits in children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47:1469–1483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mann V., Shankweiler D., Smith S. (1984). The association between comprehension of spoken sentences and early reading ability: The role of phonetic representations. Journal of Child Language 11:627–643Google Scholar
  34. Meyer B., Freedle R. (1984). Effects of discourse type on recall. American Educational Research Journal 21:121–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Miller J., Chapman R. (2001). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (v.7). Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WIGoogle Scholar
  36. Morris N., Crump W. (1982). Syntactic and vocabulary development in the written language of learning disabled and non-disabled students at four age levels. Learning Disability Quarterly 5:163–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nelson N. W., Bahr C., Van Meter A. (2004). The writing lab approach to language instruction and intervention. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc., Baltimore, MDGoogle Scholar
  38. Nelson N. W., Van Meter (2002). Assessing curriculum-based reading and writing samples. Topics in Language Disorders 22:35–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Paul R. (2001). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Assessment and intervention. (2nd ed.). Mosby, St. Louis, MOGoogle Scholar
  40. Ramus F., Rosen S., Dakin S.C., Day B.L., Castellote J.M., White S., Frith U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 126:841–865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rispens J. (2004). Syntactic and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen, NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
  42. Rispens J., Roeleven S., Koster C. (2004). Sensitivity to subject–verb agreement in spoken language in children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics 17:333–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Scott C. (1989). Problem writers: Nature, assessment, and intervention. In: Kamhi A., Catts H. (eds) Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, pp. 303–344Google Scholar
  44. Scott C. (1999). Learning to write. In: Catts H., Kamhi A. (eds) Language and reading disabilities. Allyn & Bacon, Needham Heights, MA, pp. 224–258Google Scholar
  45. Scott C., Stokes S. (1995). Measures of syntax in school-age children and adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 26:309–317Google Scholar
  46. Scott C., Windsor J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written discourse produced by school-age children with and without language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43:324–339Google Scholar
  47. Shaywitz S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia. Alfred A. Knopf, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  48. Silliman E. R., Butler K. G., Wallach G. P. (2003). The time has come to talk of many things. In: Butler K. G., Silliman E. R. (eds) Speaking, reading, and writing in children with language learning disabilities: New paradigms in research and practice. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 3–25Google Scholar
  49. Snowling M. (1991). Developmental reading disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32:49–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Snowling M. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Blackwell, Oxford, EnglandGoogle Scholar
  51. Stanovich K., Siegel L. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology 86:24–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stein C., Cairns H., Zurif E. (1984). Sentence comprehension limitations limited to syntactic deficits in reading disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics 5:305–321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tallal P., Allard L., Miller S., Curtiss S. (1997). Academic outcomes of language impaired children. In: Hulme C., Snowling M. (eds) Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention. Whurr, London, pp. 167–181Google Scholar
  54. Torgesen J. K. (1985). Memory processes in reading disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities 18:350–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Torgesen J. K., Wagner R. K., Rashotte C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Pro-Ed, Austin, TXGoogle Scholar
  56. Treiman R. (1997). Spelling in normal children and dyslexics. In: Blachman B. (eds) Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 219–218Google Scholar
  57. Treiman R., Kessler B. (2005). Writing systems and spelling development. In: Snowling M. J., Hulme C. (eds) The science of reading: A handbook. Blackwell Publishing, Malden, MA, pp. 120–134Google Scholar
  58. Van der Lely H. J. K. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua 98:243–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wagner R., Torgesen J., Rashotte C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Pro-Ed, Austin, TXGoogle Scholar
  60. Ward-Lonergan J., Liles B., Anderson A. (1999). Verbal retelling abilities in adolescents with and without language-learning disabilities for social studies lectures. Journal of Learning Disabilities 32:213–223Google Scholar
  61. Watkins R., Kelly D., Harbers H., Hollis W. (1995). Measuring children’s lexical diversity: Differentiating typical and impaired language learners. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38:1349–1355Google Scholar
  62. Weiderholt J., Bryant B. (2001). Gray oral reading mastery test-4. Pro-Ed, Austin, TXGoogle Scholar
  63. Wilkinson G. (1993). Wide range achievement test-3. Jastak Associates, Wilmington, DEGoogle Scholar
  64. Windsor J., Scott C., Street C. (2000). Verb and noun morphology in spoken and written language of children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 43:1322–1336Google Scholar
  65. Woodcock R. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test-revised. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MNGoogle Scholar
  66. Woodcock R., Mather N. (1989). Woodcock Johnson R tests of cognitive abilities-revised. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MNGoogle Scholar
  67. Woodcock R., McGrew K., Mather N. (2002). Woodcock Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Riverside Publishing, Itasca, ILGoogle Scholar
  68. Woods M. L, Moe A. J. (1989). Analytical reading inventory (4th ed.). Macmillan Publishing Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cynthia S. Puranik
    • 1
  • Linda J. Lombardino
    • 1
  • Lori J. Altmann
    • 1
  1. 1.Communication Sciences & DisordersUniversity of FloridaGainesvilleUnited States

Personalised recommendations