Quality & Quantity

, Volume 48, Issue 6, pp 3289–3309 | Cite as

The dynamics of social agreement according to Conceptual Agreement Theory

Article

Abstract

Many social phenomena can be viewed as processes in which individuals in social groups develop agreement (e.g., public opinion, the spreading of rumor, the formation of social and linguistic conventions). Conceptual Agreement Theory (CAT) models social agreement as a simplified communicational event in which an Observer \((O)\) and Actor \((A)\) exchange ideas about a concept \(C\), and where \(O\) uses that information to infer whether \(A\)’s conceptual state is the same as its own (i.e., to infer agreement). Agreement may be true (when \(O\) infers that \(A\) is thinking \(C\) and this is in fact the case, event \(a1\)) or illusory (when \(O\) infers that \(A\) is thinking \(C\) and this is not the case, event \(a2\)). In CAT, concepts that afford \(a1\) or \(a2\) become more salient in the minds of members of social groups. Results from an agent-based model (ABM) and probabilistic model that implement CAT show that, as our conceptual analyses suggested would be the case, the simulated social system selects concepts according to their usefulness to agents in promoting agreement among them (Experiment 1). Furthermore, the ABM exhibits more complex dynamics where similar minded agents cluster and are able to retain useful concepts even when a different group of agents discards them (Experiment 2). We discuss the relevance of CAT and the current findings for analyzing different social communication events, and suggest ways in which CAT could be put to empirical test.

Keywords

Conceptual Agreement Theory Agent-based modeling Conceptual diversity Dynamics of conceptual development 

References

  1. Aaker, D.A.: Building Strong Brands. The Free Press, New York (1996)Google Scholar
  2. Alvesson, M.: Knowledge Work and Knowledge-Intensive Firms. Oxford University Press, New York (2004)Google Scholar
  3. Angell, A.: Party change in Chile in comparative perspective. Rev. Cienc. Política 23(2), 88–108 (2003)Google Scholar
  4. Arrow, H., McGrath, J.E., Berdahl, J.L.: Small Groups as Complex Systems: Formation, Coordination, Development and Adaptation. Sage, London (2000)Google Scholar
  5. Axelrod, R.: Advancing the art of simulation in the social sciences. In: Conte, R., Hegselmann, R., Terna, P. (eds.) Simulating Social Phenomena, pp. 21–41. Springer, Berlin (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Ball, P.: The physical modelling of human social systems. Complexus 1, 190–206 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barsalou, L.W.: The instability of graded structure: implications for the nature of concepts. In: Neisser, U. (ed.) Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization, pp. 101–140. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1987)Google Scholar
  8. Barsalou, L.W.: Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In: Collins, A.C., Gathercole, S.E., Conway, M.A. (eds.) Theories of Memory, pp. 29–101. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London (1993)Google Scholar
  9. Berthon, P., Leyland, F.P., Campbell, C.: Does brand meaning exist in similarity or singularity? J. Bus. Res. 62(3), 356–361 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brewer, M.B.: A dual process model of impression formation. In: Srull, T.K., Wyer Jr, R.S. (eds.) Advances in Social Cognition, pp. 1–36. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (1988)Google Scholar
  11. Brown, S.P., Kozinets, R.V., Sherry Jr, J.F.: Teaching old brands new tricks: retro branding and the revival of brand meaning. J. Mark. 67(3), 19–33 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Canessa, E., Riolo, R.: An agent-based model of the impact of computer-mediated communication on organizational culture and performance: an example of the application of complex systems analysis tools to the study of CIS. J. Inf. Technol. 21, 272–283 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Canessa, E., Chaigneau, S.E., Quezada, A.: An ABM of the development of shared meaning in a social group. In: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence, Rome, Italy, 28–30 January, pp. 5–14 (2011)Google Scholar
  14. Canessa, E., Chaigneau, S.E., Quezada, A.: The life of concepts: an ABM of conceptual drift in social groups. In: Filipe, J., Fred, A. (eds.) Agents and Artificial Intelligence. Communications in Computer and Information Science Series, vol. 271, pp. 271–286. Springer, Heidelberg (2012)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Castellano, C., Fortunato, S., Loreto, V.: Statistical physics of social dynamics. Rev. Mod. Phys. 81(2), 591–646 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chaigneau, S.E., Canessa, E.: The power of collective action: how agents get rid of useless concepts without even noticing their futility. Actas Jornadas Chilenas de Computación, Curicó, Chile 7–11 November pp. 1–8 (2011)Google Scholar
  17. Chaigneau, S.E., Canessa, E., Gaete, J.: Conceptual agreement theory. New Ideas Psychol. 30(2), 179–189 (2012)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Converse, P.E.: The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In: Apter, D.E. (ed.) Ideology and Discontent, pp. 206–261. The Free Press, New York (1964)Google Scholar
  19. Cova, B., Pace, S.: Brand community of convenience products: new forms of customer empowerment—the case “My Nutella the community”. Eur. J. Mark. 40(9/10), 1087–2005 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. D’Lauro, C., Tanaka, J.W., Curran, T.: The preferred level of face categorization depends on discriminability. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15, 623–629 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ereshefsky, M.: Species and the Linnaean hierarchy. In: Wilson, R.A. (ed.) Species: New Interdisciplinary Essays, pp. 285–306. MIT Press, Cambridge (2000)Google Scholar
  22. Evans, J.: Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 255–278 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fournier, S.: Consumers and their brands: developing relationship theory in consumer research. J. Consum. Res. 24(4), 343–373 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Frege, G.: On sense and reference. In: Geach, P., Black, M. (eds.), Translations from the Philosophical Writings of Gottlob Frege (pp. 56–78). Blackwell, Oxford (1893/1952)Google Scholar
  25. Gladwin, T.N., Kennelly, J.J., Krause, T.S.: Shifting paradigms for sustainable development: implications for management theory and research. Acad. Manag. Rev. 20(4), 874–907 (1995)Google Scholar
  26. Glock, H.J.: Concepts: where subjectivism goes wrong. Philosophy 84(1), 5–29 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hampton, J.A.: Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 18, 441–461 (1979)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Keller, K.L.: Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. J. Mark. 57, 1–22 (1993)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Keller, K.L.: Strategic Brand Management: Building, Measuring, and Managing Brand Equity, 2nd edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River (2003)Google Scholar
  30. Lassar, W., Mittal, B., Sharma, A.: Measuring customer-based brand equity. J. Consum. Mark. 12(4), 11–9 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lenton, A.P., Blair, I.V., Hastie, R.: Illusions of gender: stereotypes evoke false memories. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37(1), 3–14 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lewis, D.: Convention: A Philosophical Study. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1969)Google Scholar
  33. Lewis, D.: Languages and language. In: Gunderson, K. (ed.) Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. VII, pp. 3–35. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis (1975)Google Scholar
  34. Luce, R.D.: Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis. Wiley, New York (1959)Google Scholar
  35. Luce, R.D.: The choice axiom after twenty years. J. Math. Psychol. 15(3), 215–233 (1977)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Marchione, E., Salgado, M., Gilbert, N.: ‘What did you say?’ Emergent communication in a multi-agent spatial configuration. Adv. Complex Syst. 13(4), 469–482 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mayden, R.L.: A hierarchy of species concepts: the denoument in the saga of the species problem. In: Claridge, M.F., Dawah, H.A., Wilson, M.R. (eds.) Species: The Units of Diversity, pp. 381–423. Chapman and Hall, London (1997)Google Scholar
  38. McCracken, G.: Culture and consumption: a theoretical account of the structure and movement of the cultural meaning of consumer goods. J. Consum. Res. 13, 71–84 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McRae, K., Cree, G.S., Seidenberg, M.S., McNorgan, C.: Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behav. Res. Methods 37, 547–559 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Millikan, R.G.: Language: A Biological Model. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2005)Google Scholar
  41. Mishler, B.: Getting rid of species? In: Wilson, R.A. (ed.) Species: New Interdisciplinary Essays, pp. 307–315. MIT Press, Cambridge (2000)Google Scholar
  42. Murphy, G.L., Brownell, H.H.: Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 11(1), 70–84 (1985)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nairn, A., Griffin, C., Wicks, P.G.: Children’s use of brand symbolism: a consumer culture theory approach. Eur. J. Mark. 42(5/6), 627–640 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Patalano, A.L., Chin-Parker, S., Ross, B.H.: The importance of being coherent: category coherence, cross-classification, and reasoning. J. Mem. Lang. 54(3), 407–424 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pleijel, F., Rouse, G.W.: A new taxon, capricornia (Hesionidae, Polychaeta), illustrating the LITU (‘least-inclusive taxonomic unit’) concept. Zool. Scr. 29, 157–168 (2000a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Pleijel, F., Rouse, G.W.: Least-inclusive taxonomic unit: a new taxonomic concept for biology. Proc. R. Soc. B 267, 627–630 (2000b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Robinson, J.: Squaring the circle? Some thoughts on the idea of sustainable development. Ecol. Econ. 48(4), 369–384 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rogers, T.T., Patterson, K.: Object categorization: reversals and explanations of the basic-level advantage. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136(3), 451–469 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rosch, E., Mervis, C.B.: Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 573–605 (1975)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rosch, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W.D., Johnson, D.M., Boyes-Braem, P.: Basic objects in natural categories. Cogn. Psychol. 8, 382–439 (1976)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rudman, L.A., Phelan, J.E.: Backlash effects for disconfirming gender stereotypes in organizations. Res. Organ Behav. 28, 61–79 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith, E.E.: Theories of semantic memory. In: Estes, W.K. (ed.) Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes, vol. 6, pp. 1–56. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1978)Google Scholar
  53. van Overschelde, J.P., Rawson, K.A., Dunlosky, J.: Category norms: an updated and expanded version of the norms. J. Mem. Lang. 50(3), 289–335 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vogt, P., Coumans, H.: Investigating social interaction strategies for bootstrapping lexicon development. J. Artif. Soc. Simul. 6(1) (2003). http://jasss.soc.surrey.ac.uk/6/1/4.html
  55. Wittgenstein, L.: Philosophical Investigations. Blackwell, Oxford (1953)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Facultad de Ingeniería y CienciasUniversidad Adolfo IbáñezViña del MarChile
  2. 2.Escuela de PsicologíaUniversidad Adolfo IbáñezSantiagoChile

Personalised recommendations