Advertisement

Public Choice

, Volume 153, Issue 1–2, pp 235–249 | Cite as

Correcting mistakes: cognitive dissonance and political attitudes in Sweden and the United States

  • Mikael ElinderEmail author
Article

Abstract

Cognitive dissonance theory predicts that the act of voting makes people more positive toward the party or candidate they have voted for. Following Mullainathan and Washington (Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 1:86–111, 2009), I test this prediction by using exogenous variation in turnout provided by the voting age restriction. I improve on previous studies by investigating political attitudes, measured just before elections, when they are highly predictive of voting. In contrast to earlier studies I find no effect of voting on political attitudes. This result holds for both Sweden and the United States.

Keywords

Cognitive dissonance Voting Elections Political attitudes 

JEL Classification

B59 C21 D72 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Supplementary material

11127_2011_9791_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (98 kb)
Supplementary material to: “Correcting mistakes: Cognitive dissonance and political attitudes in Sweden and the United States” (PDF 94 KB)

References

  1. Akerlof, G. A. (1989). The economics of illusion. Economics and Politics, 1, 1–15. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Akerlof, G. A., & Dickens, W. T. (1982). The economic consequences of cognitive dissonance. American Economic Review, 72, 307–319. Google Scholar
  3. Alford, J. R., Funk, C. L., & Hibbing, J. R. (2005). Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99, 153–167. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, C., Mendes, S., & Tverdova, Y. (2004). Endogenous economic voting: evidence from the 1997 British election. Electoral Studies, 23, 683–708. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aronson, E. (1968). Dissonance theory: progress and problems. In R. P. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: a sourcebook (pp. 5–27). Chicago: Rand McNally. Google Scholar
  6. Aronson, E. (1992). The return of the repressed: dissonance theory makes a comeback. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 303–311. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beasley, R., & Joslyn, M. (2001). Cognitive dissonance and post-decision attitude change in six presidential elections. Political Psychology, 22, 521–540. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boeri, T., & Tabellini, G. (2010, in press). Does information increase political support for pension reform? Public Choice. doi: 10.1007/s11127-010-9706-6.
  9. Brennan, G., & Lomasky, L. (1993). Democracy and decision—the pure theory of electoral preferences. New York: Cambridge University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Caplan, B. (2001). Rational ignorance versus rational irrationality. Kykols, 54, 3–26. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caplan, B. (2002). Systematically biased beliefs about economics: robust evidence of content-dependent judgmental anomalies from the Survey of Americans and Economists on the Economy. Economic Journal, 112, 1–26. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caplan, B. (2006). How do voters form positive economic beliefs? Evidence from the Survey of Americans and Economists on the Economy. Public Choice, 128, 367–381. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chen, K., & Risen, J. (2010). How choice affects and reflects preferences: revisiting the free-choice paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 573–594. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Congelton, R. (2001). Rational ignorance, rational voter expectations, and public policy: a discrete informational foundation for fiscal illusion. Public Choice, 107, 35–64. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cooper, J. (1980). Reducing fears and increasing assertiveness: the role of dissonance reduction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 354–363. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cooper, J. (2007). Cognitive dissonance: fifty years of a classic theory. London: Sage. Google Scholar
  17. Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–264). Orlando: Academic Press. Google Scholar
  18. Della Vigna, S., & Kaplan, E. (2007). The fox news effect: media bias and voting. Quarterly Journal of Economic, 122, 1187–1234. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. Boston: Addison-Wesley. Google Scholar
  20. Egan, L. C., Santos, L. R., & Bloom, P. (2007). The origins of cognitive dissonance: evidence from children and monkeys. Psychological Science, 18, 978–983. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Google Scholar
  22. Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1964). When prophecy fails: a social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the destruction of the world. New York: Harper. Google Scholar
  23. Gerber, A. S., Karlan, D., & Bergan, D. (2009). Does the media matter? A field experiment measuring the effect of newspapers on voting behavior and political opinions. American Economic Journal—Applied Economics, 1, 35–52. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (Eds.) (1999). Cognitive dissonance: progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington: American Psychological Association. Google Scholar
  25. Holmberg, S., & Oscarsson, H. (2004). Väljare. In Svenskt väljarbeteende under 50 år. Stockholm: Nordstedts Juridik AB. Google Scholar
  26. Jordahl, H. (2006). An economic analysis of voting in Sweden. Public Choice, 127, 251–265. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Konow, J. (2000). Fair shares: accountability and cognitive dissonance in allocation decisions. American Economic Review, 90, 1072–1091. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lewis-Beck, M. S. (2006). Does economics still matter? Econometrics and the vote. Journal of Politics, 68, 208–212. Google Scholar
  29. Masters, J. (2005). A midwestern response to the events of September 11, 2001. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 10, 369–381. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mullainathan, S., & Washington, E. (2009). Sticking with your vote: cognitive dissonance and political attitudes. American Economic Journal—Applied Economics, 1, 86–111. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Östling, R. (2009). Economic influences on moral values. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis and Policy, 9, Advances, Article 2. Google Scholar
  32. Rabin, M. (1994). Cognitive dissonance and social change. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 23, 177–194. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stone, J., Aronson, E., Crain, A. L., Winslow, M. P., & Fried, C. B. (1994). Inducing hypocrisy as a means of encouraging young adults to use condoms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 116–128. CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Research Institute of Industrial Economics (IFN) and Uppsala Center for Fiscal Studies (UCFS)Uppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden
  2. 2.Department of EconomicsUppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden

Personalised recommendations