Political Behavior

, Volume 38, Issue 1, pp 155–178

How Disgust Influences Health Purity Attitudes

Original Paper

Abstract

Food and health regulations are increasingly being pushed onto the political agenda, with rising concerns about genetically modified foods, obesity rates, and vaccination. Public beliefs and attitudes on these issues often conflict with the scientific evidence, yet we know relatively little about what influences opinion on these issues. The public lacks clear partisan cues, and many food and health attitudes cut across the ideological spectrum. We argue that these issues represent new ‘purity’ attitudes that are driven by the emotion of disgust. Across three studies, both by measuring individuals’ trait disgust sensitivity and experimentally inducing an emotional state of disgust, we demonstrate the impact of disgust on food and health policy attitudes. Our results show that greater sensitivity to disgust is associated with support for organic foods, opposition to genetically modified foods, and anti-vaccination beliefs. However, we find only limited evidence that experimentally manipulated disgust affects attitudes toward genetically modified and organic foods. Overall, our results demonstrate that disgust plays an important role in attitudes regarding public health and broadens our understanding of purity attitudes.

Keywords

Disgust Emotion Health Purity Food Natural Organic Vaccination 

Supplementary material

11109_2015_9310_MOESM1_ESM.docx (946 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 945 kb)

References

  1. Ahler, D. J. (2014). Self-fulfilling misperceptions of public polarization. The Journal of Politics, 76(03), 607–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Balzer, A., & Jacobs, C. M. (2011). Gender and physiological effects in connecting disgust to political preferences. Social Science Quarterly,. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00819.x.Google Scholar
  3. Banks, A. J., & Valentino, N. A. (2012). Emotional substrates of white racial attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 286–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barry, C. L., Brescoll, V. L., Brownell, K. D., & Schlesinger, M. (2009). Obesity metaphors: How beliefs about the causes of obesity affect support for public policy. The Milbank Quarterly, 87(1), 7–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Belch, M. A., & Willis, L. A. (2002). Family decision at the turn of the century: Has the changing structure of households impacted the family decision-making process? Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 2(2), 111–124. doi:10.1002/cb.94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368. doi:10.1093/pan/mpr057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brader, T. (2005). Striking a responsive chord: How political ads motivate and persuade voters by appealing to emotions. American Journal of Political Science, 49(2), 388–405. doi:10.1111/j.0092-5853.2005.00130.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00353.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brownell, K. D. (1991). Personal responsibility and control over our bodies: When expectation exceeds reality. Health Psychology, 10(5), 303–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cashdan, E., & Steele, M. (2013). Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample. Human Nature, 24(1), 59–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cassese, E. C., & Hannagan, R. (2014). Framing and women’s support for government spending on breast cancer research and treatment programs. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy,. doi:10.1111/asap.12048.Google Scholar
  12. Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323(5918), 1222–1226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Charash, M., & McKay, D. (2002). Attention bias for disgust. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(5), 529–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clifford, S., & Jerit, J. (2013). How words do the work of politics: Moral foundations theory and the debate over stem cell research. The Journal of Politics, 1–13.Google Scholar
  15. Clifford, S., Jerit, J., Rainey, C., & Motyl, M. (2015). Moral concerns and policy attitudes: Investigating the influence of elite rhetoric. Political Communication, 33(2), 229–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Costa-Font, M., Gil, J. M., & Traill, W. B. (2008). Consumer acceptance, valuation of and attitudes towards genetically modified food: Review and implications for food policy. Food Policy, 33(2), 99–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Curtis, V., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44(1), 17–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 333–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychological Science, 24(1), 56–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings. Biological sciences/The Royal Society, 275(1640), 1279–1285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gadarian, S. K. (2010). The politics of threat: How terrorism news shapes foreign policy attitudes. The Journal of Politics, 72(02), 469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gadarian, S. K., & Albertson, B. (2014). Anxiety, immigration, and the search for information. Political Psychology, 35(2), 133–164. doi:10.1111/pops.12034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gault, B. A., & Sabini, J. (2000). The roles of empathy, anger, and gender in predicting attitudes toward punitive, reparative, and preventative public policies. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 495–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gerber, A. S., et al. (2011). Personality Traits and Participation in Political Processes. The Journal of Politics, 73(3), 692–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Groenendyk, E. (2011). Current emotion research in political science: how emotions help democracy overcome its collective action problem. Emotion Review, 3(4), 455–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98–116. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hatemi, P., & McDermott, R. (2012). Policing the perimeter: Disgust and purity in democratic debate. Political Science & Politics, 45(04), 675–687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hatemi, P. K., et al. (2013). Fear as a disposition and an emotional state: A genetic and environmental approach to out-group political preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 57(2), 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hauser, D. J., & Schwarz, N. (2015). Attentive turkers: MTurk participants perform better on online attention checks than do subject pool participants. Behavior Research Methods.Google Scholar
  32. Hausman, B. L., Ghebremichael, M., Hayek, P., & Mack, E. (2014). ‘Poisonous, filthy, loathsome, damnable stuff’: The rhetorical ecology of vaccination concern. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 87(4), 403–416.Google Scholar
  33. Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 297–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963–976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition & Emotion, 23(4), 714–725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2012). Disgusting smells cause decreased liking of gay men. Emotion, 12(1), 23–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2011). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kahan, D. M., et al. (2012). The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nature Climate Change, 2(10), 732–735. doi:10.1038/nclimate1547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Koleva, S. P., et al. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kurzban, R., Dukes, A., & Weeden, J. (2010). Sex, drugs and moral goals: Reproductive strategies and views about recreational drugs. Proceedings. Biological sciences/The Royal Society, 277(1699), 3501–3508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lee, J. J., Sohn, Y., & Fowler, J. H. (2013). Emotion regulation as the foundation of political attitudes: Does reappraisal decrease support for conservative policies? PloS One, 8(12), 83143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lieberman, D., & Patrick, C. (2014). Are the behavioral immune system and pathogen disgust identical? Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 244–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lieberman, D. L., Tybur, J. M., & Latner, J. D. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender: Contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity, 20(9), 1803–1814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mäkiniemi, J.-P., Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M., & Pieri, M. (2012). The endorsement of the moral foundations in food-related moral thinking in three European Countries. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 26(4), 771–786. doi:10.1007/s10806-012-9401-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & Michael, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  47. Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2012). Overperceiving disease cues: The basic cognition of the behavioral immune system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1198–1213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the Foundations of Political Behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Mondak, J. J., et al. (2010). Personality and Civic Engagement: An Integrative Framework for the Study of Trait Effects on Political Behavior. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 270–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(4), 1042–1051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Nicolia, A., Manzo, A., Veronesi, F., & Rosellini, D. (2014). An overview of the last 10 years of genetically engineered crop safety research. Critical Reviews in Biotechnology, 34(1), 77–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nisbet, M. C., Brossard, D., & Kroepsch, A. (2003). Framing science: The stem cell controversy in an age of PressPolitics. The Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 8(2), 36–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2014). Does correcting myths about the flu vaccine work? An experimental evaluation of the effects of corrective information. Vaccine, 33(3), 459–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Nyhan, B., Reifler, J., Richey, S., & Freed, G. L. (2014). Effective messages in vaccine promotion: a randomized trial. Pediatrics, 133(4), e835–e842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Olatunji, B. O., et al. (2007). The disgust scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Olatunji, B. O., et al. (2008). Disgust, scrupulosity and conservative attitudes about sex: Evidence for a mediational model of homophobia. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(5), 1364–1369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Olatunji, B. O., et al. (2012). The three domains of disgust scale: Factor structure, psychometric properties, and conceptual limitations. Assessment, 19(2), 205–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Oxley, D. R., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321(5896), 1667–1670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Panagopoulos, C. (2011). Thank you for voting: Gratitude expression and voter mobilization. The Journal of Politics, 73(03), 707–717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Park, J. H., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2003). Evolved disease-avoidance processes and contemporary anti-social behavior: prejudicial attitudes and avoidance of people with physical disabilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(2), 65–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Park, J. H., Schaller, M., & Crandall, C. S. (2007). Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(6), 410–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Petersen, M. B. (2010). Distinct emotions, distinct domains: anger, anxiety and perceptions of intentionality. The Journal of Politics, 72(02), 357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Petersen, M. B., Sznycer, D., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2012). Who deserves help? Evolutionary psychology, social emotions, and public opinion about welfare. Political psychology, 33(3), 395–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rasmussen, M., et al. (2006). Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: A review of the literature. Part I: Quantitative studies. The. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 3(1), 22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Richards, J. L., et al. (2013). Nonmedical exemptions to immunization requirements in California: A 16-year longitudinal analysis of trends and associated community factors. Vaccine, 31(29), 3009–3013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Rozin, P. (1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science, 10(3), 218–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 23–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & Fincher, K. (2009). From oral to moral. Science, 323(5918), 1179–1180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: The transformation of preferences into values and the recruitment of disgust. Psychological Science, 8(2), 67–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 703–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Rozin, P., et al. (1999). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity: Comparisons and evaluations of paper-and-pencil versus behavioral measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 33(3), 330–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Rozin, P., et al. (2004). Preference for natural: Instrumental and ideational/moral motivations, and the contrast between foods and medicines. Appetite, 43(2), 147–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Rozin, P., et al. (2005). The meaning of ‘natural’: Process more important than content. Psychological science, 16(8), 652–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2010). Moral anger is more flexible than moral disgust. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(4), 360–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2011). Social justifications for moral emotions: When reasons for disgust are less elaborated than for anger. Emotion, 11(3), 637–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Ryan, T. J. (2012). What makes us click? Demonstrating incentives for angry discourse with digital-age field experiments. The Journal of Politics, 74(04), 1138–1152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Sawchuk, C. N., et al. (2000). Disgust sensitivity and contamination fears in spider and blood–injection–injury phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(8), 753–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 212–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2), 99–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096–1109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Searles, K., and Mattes, K. 2015. It’s a mad, mad world: Using emotion inductions in a survey. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 1–11.Google Scholar
  84. Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception? Risk Analysis, 2(2), 83–93. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01369.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149–168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00621.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Smith, K. B., et al. (2011a). Disgust Sensitivity and the neurophysiology of left-right political orientations. PloS One, 6(10), 25552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Smith, P. J., et al. (2011b). Parental delay or refusal of vaccine doses, childhood vaccination coverage at 24 months of age, and the health belief model. Public health reports, 126(2), 135–146.Google Scholar
  88. Strohminger, N. (2014). Disgust talked about. Philosophy Compass, 9(7), 478–493. doi:10.1111/phc3.12137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Sugerman, D. E., et al. (2010). Measles outbreak in a highly vaccinated population, San Diego, 2008: Role of the intentionally undervaccinated. Pediatrics, 125(4), 747–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Tenbült, P., de Vries, N. K., Dreezens, E., & Martijn, C. (2005). Perceived naturalness and acceptance of genetically modified food. Appetite, 45(1), 47–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 99–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 587–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Tiggemann, M., & Rothblum, E. D. (1988). Gender differences in social consequences of perceived overweight in the United States and Australia. Sex Roles, 18(1–2), 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Toronchuk, J. A., & Ellis, G. F. R. (2007). Disgust: Sensory affect or primary emotional system? Cognition & Emotion, 21(8), 1799–1818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Tybur, J. M., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Pollet, T. V. (2014). Behavioral immune system methods: Surveying the present to shape the future. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 274–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Tybur, J. M., et al. (2010). Extending the behavioral immune system to political psychology: are political conservatism and disgust sensitivity really related? Evolutionary Psychology: An International Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Psychology and Behavior, 8(4), 599–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Tybur, J. M., et al. (2011). Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 343–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Valentino, N. A., Banks, A. J., Hutchings, V. L., & Davis, A. K. (2009). Selective exposure in the internet age: The interaction between anxiety and information utility. Political Psychology, 30(4), 591–613. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2009.00716.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Valentino, N. A., Gregorowicz, K., & Groenendyk, E. W. (2008). Efficacy, emotions and the habit of participation. Political Behavior, 31(3), 307–330. doi:10.1007/s11109-008-9076-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Valentino, N. A., et al. (2011). Election night’s alright for fighting: The role of emotions in political participation. The Journal of Politics, 73(01), 156–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Vartanian, L. R. (2010). Disgust and perceived control in attitudes toward obese people. International Journal of Obesity (2005), 34(8), 1302–1307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Vermeir, I., & Verbeke, W. (2006). Sustainable food consumption: Exploring the consumer ‘attitude–behavioral intention’ gap. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 19(2), 169–194. doi:10.1007/s10806-005-5485-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Wang, E., Clymer, J., Davis-Hayes, C., & Buttenheim, A. (2014). Nonmedical exemptions from school immunization requirements: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health, 104(11), 62–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Warren, C. W., Jones, N. R., Eriksen, M. P., & Asma, S. (2006). Patterns of global tobacco use in young people and implications for future chronic disease burden in adults. Lancet, 367(9512), 749–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Weinberg, J. D., Freese, J., & McElhattan, D. (2014). Comparing data characteristics and results of an online factorial survey between a population-based and crowdsource-recruited sample. Sociological Science, 1, 292–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Political ScienceUniversity of HoustonHoustonUSA
  2. 2.Department of Political ScienceLoyola UniversityChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations