Political Behavior

, Volume 32, Issue 4, pp 517–545 | Cite as

Explicit Evidence on the Import of Implicit Attitudes: The IAT and Immigration Policy Judgments

ORIGINAL PAPER

Abstract

The implicit association test (IAT) is increasingly used to detect automatic attitudes. Yet a fundamental question remains about this measure: How well can it predict individual judgments? Though studies find that IAT scores shape individual evaluations, these inquiries do not account for an array of well-validated, theoretically relevant variables, thus raising the challenge of omitted variable bias. For scholars using the IAT, the risk here is one of misattributing to implicit attitudes what can be better explained by alternate and rigorous self-reports of explicit constructs. This paper examines the IAT’s performance in the context of U.S. immigration politics. Using a representative web survey of adults, I demonstrate the IAT effectively captures implicit attitude toward Latino immigrants. Critically, I then show these attitudes substantively mold individual preferences for illegal and legal immigration policy, net of political ideology, socio-economic concerns, and well-established measures of intolerance toward immigrants, such as authoritarianism and ethnocentrism. Combined, these results suggest the IAT measures attitudes that are non-redundant and potent predictors of individual political judgments.

Keywords

Implicit association test (IAT) Implicit attitudes Automaticity Immigration Latino immigrants 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I thank John Aldrich, Kerry Haynie, Vince Hutchings, and Paula McClain for their guidance on an early version of this manuscript. I am also grateful for the feedback and encouragement on this project provided by Amada Armenta, John Geer, Marc Hetherington, Natalie Masuoka, and Liz Zechmeister. Finally, I am particularly indebted to Cindy Kam, who patiently read and commented on multiple drafts of this article. Any remaining errors are, of course, my responsibility alone.

References

  1. Alba, R., & Nee, V. (2003). Remaking the American Mainstream: Assimilation and contemporary immigration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Albertson, B. (2008). Religious appeals and implicit attitudes. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  3. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  4. Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Arcuri, L., Castelli, L., Galdi, S., Zogmaister, Cristina., & Amadori, A. (2008). Predicting the vote: Implicit attitudes as predictors of the future behavior of decided and undecided voters. Political Psychology, 29, 369–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or “would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the implicit association test?”. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 257–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Ashburn-Nardo, L., Knowles, M. L., & Monteith, M. J. (2003). Black Americans’ implicit racial associations and their implications for intergroup judgment. Social Cognition, 21, 61–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Banaji, M., Blair, I. V., & Schwarz, N. (1995). Implicit memory and survey measurement. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Answering questions: Methodology for determining cognitive and communicative processes in survey research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  9. Banaji, M., Lemm, K. M., & Carpenter, S. J. (2001). The social unconscious. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes. Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  10. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Berrens, R. P., Bohara, A. K., Jenkins-Smith, H., Silva, C., & Weiner, D. L. (2003). The advent of internet surveys for political research: A comparison of telephone and internet samples. Political Analysis, 11, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and the immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52, 959–978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Brown, T. A. (2007). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  16. Burdein, I., Lodge, M., & Taber, C. (2006). Experiments on the automaticity of political beliefs and attitudes. Political Psychology, 27, 359–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chavez, L. R. (2001). Covering immigration: Popular images and the politics of the nation. Berkeley: UC Press.Google Scholar
  18. Citrin, J., Green, D. P., Muste, C., & Wong, C. (1997). Public opinion toward immigration reform: the role of economic motivations. Journal of Politics, 59, 858–881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American = White? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Falkenstein, M. R. (2002). The Asian and Pacific Islander surname list: As developed from Census 2000. Working Paper. U.S. Census Bureau.Google Scholar
  23. Fazio, R. H., Chen, J.-M., McDonel, E. C., & Sherman, S. J. (1982). Attitude accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency, and the strength of the object-evaluation association. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 339–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  28. Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24, 41–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Fiedler, K., & Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the implicit association tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology.Google Scholar
  30. Finney, S. J., & DiStefano, C. (2006). Non-normal and categorical data in structural equation modeling. In G. R. Hancock & M. O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course. Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  31. Gawronski, B., Geschke, D., & Banse, R. (2003). Implicit bias in impression formation: Associations influence the construal of individuating information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 573–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T., Andrew, U., Luis, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hattham, V. (2007). In the Shadow of Race: Jews, Latinos, and immigrant politics in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  37. Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. (2009). Divided we stand: Polarization, authoritarianism, and the contemporary political divide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Higham, J. (1981). Strangers in the land: Patterns of American nativism, 1860–1925. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Huddy, L., & Khatib, N. (2007). American patriotism, national identity, and political involvement. American Journal of Political Science, 51, 63–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Huntington, S. P. (2004, March/April). The Hispanic challenge. Foreign Policy, pp. 30–45.Google Scholar
  41. Jacobson, M. F. (1998). Whiteness of a different color: European immigrants and the alchemy of race. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Jellison, W. A., McConnell, A. R., & Gabriel, S. (2004). Implicit and explicit measures of sexual orientation attitudes: In group preferences and related behaviors and beliefs among gay and straight men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 629–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kam, C. D. (2007). Implicit attitudes, explicit choices: When subliminal priming predicts candidate preference. Political Behavior, 29, 343–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kam, C. D., & Kinder, D. R. (2007). Terror and ethnocentrism: Foundations of American support for the war on terrorism. Journal of Politics, 69, 320–338.Google Scholar
  46. Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774–788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Kim, D.-Y. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kinder, D. R., & Kam, C. D. (2009). Us against them: Ethnocentric foundations of American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  49. King, D. (2000). Making Americans: Immigration, race, and the origins of the diverse democracy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  51. Krosnick, J. A., Pasek, J., Lelkes, Y., Akhtar, O., Thompson, T., & Payne, K. (2009). The impact of racism on votes in the 2008 presidential election: Results from the Associated Press/Yahoo News!/Stanford Survey, the Stanford MRI Survey, and the American National Election Studies. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association. Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  52. Lee, Y.-T., & Ottati, V. (2002). Attitudes toward U.S. immigration policy: The role of in-group-out-group bias, economic concern, and obedience to law. The Journal of Social Psychology, 142(5), 617–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Levinson, D. J. (1950). The study of ethnocentric ideology. In T. W. Adorno, E. Frenkel-Brunswik, D. J. Levinson, et al. (Eds.), The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  54. Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Lyle, M. L. (2008). Dueling consciousness: The psychological implications of the American ideology of racial dominance. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Political Science, Duke University.Google Scholar
  56. Maison, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Bruin, R. H. (2004). Predictive validity of the implicit association test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 405–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Malhotra, N., & Krosnick, J. A. (2007). The effect of survey mode and sampling on inferences about political attitudes and behavior: Comparing the 2000 and 2004 ANES to internet surveys with nonprobability samples. Political Analysis, 15, 286–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Marcus, G. E., Sullivan, J. L., Theiss-Morse, E., & Wood, S. L. (1995). With Malice toward some: How people make civil liberties judgments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Neumann, R., Hülsenbeck, K., & Seibt, B. (2004). Attitudes towards people with AIDS and avoidance behavior: Automatic and reflective bases of behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 543–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 134, 565–584.Google Scholar
  61. Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association test. Social Cognition, 19, 625–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 101–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the implicit association test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 553–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 1–53.Google Scholar
  66. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the implicit association test: Personalizing the iat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Pérez, E. O. (2010). Immigrant = Latino: How automaticity biases immigration policy judgments. Paper presented at the Harvard University Political Psychology and Behavior Workshop, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  68. Pinter, B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2005). Clarifying the role of the “other” category in the self-esteem IAT. Experimental Psychology, 52, 74–79.Google Scholar
  69. Richeson, J. A., & Nussbaum, R. J. (2004). The impact of multiculturalism versus color-blindness on racial bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 417–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rivers, D. (2008). Understanding people: Sample matching. Palo Alto: YouGov\Polimetrix.Google Scholar
  71. Sanders, D., Clarke, H. D., Stewart, M. C., & Whitley, P. (2007). Does mode matter for modeling political choice? Evidence from the 2005 British Election Study. Political Analysis, 15, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Scheve, K. F., & Slaughter, M. J. (2001). Labor market competition and individual preferences over immigration. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 83, 133–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Simon, R. J., & Alexander, S. H. (1993). The ambivalent welcome: Print media, public opinion and immigration. Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
  75. Sniderman, P. M., Hagendoorn, L., & Prior, M. (2004). Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review, 98, 35–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Sniderman, P. M., Peri, P., de Figueiredo, R. J., & Piazza, T. (2000). The outsider: Prejudice and politics in Italy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Sniderman, P. M., & Tetlock, P. E. (1986). Symbolic racism: Problems of motive attribution in political analysis. Journal of Social Issues, 42, 129–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Waldman, P., Ventura, E., Savillo, R., Lin, S., & Lewis, G. (2008). Fear and loathing in prime time: Immigration myths and cable news. Report. Media Matters Action Network.Google Scholar
  80. Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Word, D. L., & Perkins, R. C. (1996). Building a Spanish surname list for the 1990s—a new approach to an old problem. Technical Working Paper No. 13, U.S. Census Bureau.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Political ScienceVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations