Advertisement

Olfactory imagery: is exactly what it smells like

  • Benjamin D. YoungEmail author
Article

Abstract

Mental Imagery, whereby we experience aspect of a perceptual scene or perceptual object in the absence of direct sensory stimulation is ubiquitous. Often the existence of mental imagery is demonstrated by asking one’s reader to volitionally generate a visual object, such as closing ones eyes and imagining an apple. However, mental imagery also arises in auditory, tactile, interoceptive, and olfactory cases. A number of influential philosophical theories have attempted to explain mental imagery in terms of belief-based forms of representation using the Dependence Thesis, dependence upon means of access, such as enactivism, or in terms of the similarity of content with perceptual processing. The focus of this paper concerns the later approach and in particular assessing if Nanay’s promissory note that his theory is applicable to modalities other than vision, such as smell, seems likely to be of theoretical tender. The thesis argued for in this paper is that olfactory imagery exists and is best accounted for by considering it as a type of perceptual processing with a unique representational format relative to the olfactory perceptual modality. The paper concludes by summarizing the applicability of Nanay’s theory of mental imagery for olfaction and suggests some further issues that arise when transitioning to multi-modal mental imagery.

Keywords

Mental imagery Olfactory imagery Smell Perception Olfactory quality 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Previous versions of this paper have been presented at a workshop on olfactory imagery hosted by the Centre for Philosophical Psychology at the University of Antwerp, at Jonas Olofsson research group in the Department of Psychology at Stockholm University, and at Johan Lundström’s research group in the Department of Clinical Neuroscience at Karolinska Institutet. I would like to thank all of the members of the audience on these occasions for their helpful questions and feedback. In particular, I would like to thank Barry Smith, Jonas Olofsson, Johan Lundström, Artin Arshamian, Maria Larsson, and Bence Nanay for their comments on this research project.

References

  1. Aleman, A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Bocker, K. B., & de Haan, E. H. (2000). Music training and mental imagery ability. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1664–1668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arguedas, D., Langdon, R., & Stevenson, R. (2012a). Neuropsychological characteristics associated with olfactory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,18, 799–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Arguedas, D., Stevenson, R., & Langdon, R. (2012b). Source monitoring and olfactory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,121(4), 936–943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Arshamian, A., Iannilli, E., Gerber, J. C., Willander, J., Persson, J., Seo, H., et al. (2013). The functional neuroanatomy of odor evoked autobiographical memories cued by odors and word. Neuropsychologia,51, 123–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Arshamian, A., & Larsson, M. (2014). Same same but different. Frontiers in Psychology,3, 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Arshamian, A., Olofsson, J. K., Jönsson, F. U., & Larsson, M. (2008). Sniff your way to clarity: The case of olfactory imagery. Chemosensory Perception,1, 242–246.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-008-9035-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Arshamian, A., Willander, J., & Larsson, M. (2011). Olfactory awareness is positively associated to odour memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology,23, 220–226.  https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.483226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barabasz, A. F., & Gredson, R. A. (1979). Analytic wintering-over, suggestion and transient olfactory stimulation/EEG evoked potentional and electrodermal responses. Biological Psychology,9, 285–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bensafi, B., Tillmann, B. Poncelet, J., Pryzybylski, L., & Rouby, C. (2013). Olfactory and gustatory mental imagery/modulation by sensory experience and comparison to auditory mental imagery. In S. Lacey and R. Lawson (Eds.), Multisensory imagery, vol. 77.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5879-1_5.Google Scholar
  10. Bensafi, M., Pouliot, S., & Sobel, N. (2005). Odorant-specific patterns of sniffing during imagery distinguish ‘bad’ and ‘good’ olfactory imagers. Chemical Senses, 30, 521–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bensafi, M., Sobel, N., & Khan, R. M. (2007). Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 3254–3262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bensafi, M., & Rouby, C. (2007). Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception. Chemical Senses, 32, 237–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cain, W. S., et al. (1995). Life-span development ofodor identification, learning, and olfactory sensitivity. Perception,24, 1457–1472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carlson, K. S., Gadziola, M. A., Dauster, E. S., & Wesson, D. W. (2018). Selective attention controls olfactory decisions and the neural encoding of odors. Current Biology,28, 2195–2205.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cox, R. E., & Langdon, R. A. (2016). Hypnotic olfactory hallucinations. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,64(1), 24–44.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00207144.2015.1099401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Crowder, R. G., & Schab, F. R. (1995). Imagery for odors. In R. G. Crowder & F. R. Schab (Eds.), Memory for odors (pp. 93–107). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Currie, G. (1995). Visual imagery as the simulation of vision. Mind and Language,10, 25–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Currie, G., & Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative minds: Imagination in philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dou, I., Li, Y., Geisle, M. W., & Morsella, E. (2018). Involuntary polymodal imagery involving olfaction, audition, touch, taste, and vision. Consciousness and Cognition,62, 9–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Engen, T. (1982). The perception of odors. Toronto: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  21. Flohr, E. L. R., Arshamian, A., Wieser, M. J., Hummel, C., Larsson, M., Muhlberger, A., et al. (2014). The fate of the inner nose/odor imagery in patients with olfactory loss. Neuroscience,268, 118–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gilbert, A. N. (2008). What the nose knows. New York: Crown Publishers.Google Scholar
  23. Gray, R., & Tanesini, A. (2010). Perception and action: The taste test. Philosophical Quarterly,60(241), 718–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hackländer, R. P. M., Janssen, S. M. J., & Bermeitinger, C. (2018). An in-depth review of the methods, findings, and theories associated with odor-evoked autobiographical memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1545-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Henkin, R. I., Levy, L. M., & Fordyce, A. (2013a). Taste and smell function in chronic disease. American Journal of Otolaryngology,34, 477–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Henkin, R. I., Levy, L. M., & Lin, C. S. (2000). Taste and Smell Phantoms Revealed by Brain Functional MRI (fMRI). Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography Issue,24(1), 106–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Henkin, R. I., Potolicchio, S. J., & Levy, L. M. (2013b). Olfactory hallucinations without clinical motor activity/A comparison of unirhinal with birhinal phantosmia. Brain Science,3, 1483–1553.  https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3041483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Herz, R. S. (2000). Verbal coding in olfactory versus nonolfactory cognition. Memory and Cognition,28, 957–964.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ishiguro, H. (1967). Imagination. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society Supplementary,41, 37–56.Google Scholar
  30. Keller, A. (2017). Philosophy of olfactory perception. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  31. Kind, A. (2001). Putting the image back to imagination. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research,62, 85–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Koppel, J., & Berntsen, D. (2014). The peaks of life/The differential temporal locations of the reminiscence bump across disparate cueing methods. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition,4, 66–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Koppel, J., & Rubin, D. C. (2016). Recent advances in understanding the reminiscence bump/The importance of cues in guiding recall from autobiographical memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science,25(2), 135–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Larsson, M., Oberg-Blavarg, C., & Jonsson, F. U. (2009). Bad odors stick better than good ones. Experimental Psychology,56(6), 375–380.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Larsson, M., Semb, H., Winblad, B., Amberla, K., Wahlund, L.-O., & Bäckman, L. (1999). Odor identification in normal aging and early Alzheimer’s disease: Effects of retrieval support. Neuropsychology,13, 1–7.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.13.1.47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Larsson, M., & Willander, J. (2009). Autobiographical Odor Memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1170, 318–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Larsson, M., Willander, J., Karlsson, K., & Arshamia, A. (2014). Olfactory LOVER. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1–5.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Leopold, D. (2002). Distortions of olfactory perception. Chemical Senses,27, 611–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lucien, L. M., & Henkin, R. I. (2000). Physiologically initiated and inhibited phantosmia/Cyclic unirhinal, episodic, recurrent phantosmia revealed by brain fMRI. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography Issue,24(4), 501–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lycan, W. G. (2006). Enactive intentionality. Psyche,12, 3.Google Scholar
  42. Majid, A., Speed, L., Croijmans, I., & Arshamian, A. (2017). What makes a better smeller? Perception,46, 406–430.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616688224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Martin, M. G. F. (2002). The transparency of experience. Mind and Language,17, 376–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nanay, B. (2010). Perception and imagination: Amodal perception as mental imagery. Philosophical Studies,150(2), 239–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nanay, B. (2015). Perceptual content and the content of mental imagery. Philosophical Studies,172(7), 1723–1736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nanay, B. (2016a). Hallucination as mental imagery. Journal of Consciousness Studies,23, 65–81.Google Scholar
  47. Nanay, B. (2016b). Imagination and perception. In A. Kind (Ed.), Routledge handbook of the philosophy of imagination. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
  48. Nanay, B. (2017). Sensory substitution and multimodal mental imagery. Perception,46(9), 1014–1102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Nanay, B. (2018). Multimodal mental imagery. Cortex,105, 125–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Negoias, S., Aszmann, O., Cory, I., & Hummel, T. (2013). Localization of odors can be learned. Chemical Senses,38, 553–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Noë, A. (2002). On what we see. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly,83(1), 57–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  53. Noë, A. (2005). Real presence. Philosophical Topics,33(1), 235–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Noë, A. (2006). Precis of action in perception. Psyche,12, 1.Google Scholar
  55. Noë, A. (2007). Understanding action in perception: Replies to Hickerson and Keijzer. Philosophical Psychology,20(4), 531–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Noë, A. (2009). Conscious reference. Philosophical Quarterly,59(236), 470–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Noordhof, P. (2002). Imagining objects and imagining experiences. Mind and Language,17, 426–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor approach to vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,24(5), 939–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2002). What it is like to see: A sensorimotor theory of perceptual experience. Synthese,29, 79–103.Google Scholar
  60. O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as outside memory. Canadian Journal of Philosophy,46(3), 461–488.Google Scholar
  61. Pistoia, F., Carolei, A., Iacoviello, D., Petracca, A., Sacco, S., Sarà, M., et al. (2015). EEG-detected olfactory imagery to reveal covert consciousness in minimally conscious state. Brain Injury,29(13–14), 1729–1735.  https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2015.1075251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., & Royet, J. (2012). Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers. Human Brain Mapping,33, 224–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Prinz, J. (2006). Putting the brakes on enactive perception. Psyche,12, 3.Google Scholar
  64. Royet, J. P., Delon-Martin, C., & Plailly, J. (2013). Odor mental imagery in non-experts in odors. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,7, 87.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Saive, A. L., Royet, J. P., & Plailly, J. (2014). A review on the neural bases of episodic odor memory/from laboratory-based to autobiographical approaches. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience,8, 1–13.Google Scholar
  66. Sela, L., & Sobel, N. (2010). Human olfaction: A constant state of change-blindness. Experimental Brain Research,205(1), 13–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sjölund, S., Larsson, M., Olofsson, J. K., Suebert, J., & Laukka, E. (2017). Phantom smells/Prevalence and correlates in a population-based sample of older adults. Chemical Senses,42, 309–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Smith, J. (2006). Bodily awareness, imagination and the self. European Journal of Philosophy,14, 49–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Stevenson, R. J. (2009). Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2004). Olfactory dreams/Phenomenology, relationship to volitional imagery and odor identification. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,24(1), 69–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Stevenson R. J., Case T. I., & Mahmut, M. (2007). Difficulty in evoking odor images: The role of odor naming. Memory & Cognition, 35, 578–589.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Thomas, N. J. T. (2018). Mental imagery. In N. Zalta (Eds.), The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2018/entries/mental-imagery.
  73. Willander, J., & Larsson, M. (2006). Smell your way back to childhood. J Psychonimic Bulletin and Review,13(2), 240–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Willander, J., & Larsson, M. (2007). Olfaction and emotion/the case of autobio-graphical memory. Memory and Cognition,35(7), 1659–1663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Wysocki, C. J., & Gilbert, A. N. (1989). The national geographic smell survey: Effects of age are heterogeneous. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,561, 12–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Young, B. D. (2011). Olfaction: Smelling the content of consciousness (Unpublished doctoral dissertation) City University of New York, New York.Google Scholar
  77. Young, B. D. (2012). Stinking consciousness! Journal of Consciousness Studies,19, 223–243.Google Scholar
  78. Young, B. D. (2014). Smelling phenomenal. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 713.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.0071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Young, B. D. (2015). Formative non-conceptual content. Journal of Consciousness Studies,22, 201–214.Google Scholar
  80. Young, B. D. (2016). Smelling matter. Philosophical Psychology,29, 520–534.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2015.1126814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Young, B. D. (2017). Enactivism’s last breaths. In M. Curado & S. Gouveia (Eds.), Contemporary perspective in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge Press.Google Scholar
  82. Young, B. D. (2019a). Smell’s puzzling disrepency. Mind and Language. Google Scholar
  83. Young, B. D. (2019b). Smelling molecular strucute. In D. Shottenkirk, S. Gouveia, & J. Curado (Eds.), Perception and thought. Routledge Press.Google Scholar
  84. Young, B. D. (2019c). The many problems of distal olfactory perception. In T. Cheng, O. Deroy, & C. Spence (Eds.), Spatial senses: Philosophy of perception in an age of science. Routledge Press.Google Scholar
  85. Young, B. D., Keller, A., & Rosenthal, D. (2014). Quality-space theory in olfaction. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Zach, P., Zimmelova, P., Mrzilkova, J., & Kutova, M. (2018). Difference in subjective accessibility of on demand recall of visual, taste, and olfactory memories. BioMed Research International,2018, 1–6.  https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/1630437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Zelano, C., Montag, J., Khan, R., & Sobel, N. (2009). A specialized odor memory buffer in primary olfactory cortex. PLoS ONE,4(3), e4965.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Zhou, W., & Chen, D. (2009). Binaural rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex. Current Biology,19, 1561–1565.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The Department of PhilosophyUniversity of NevadaRenoUSA

Personalised recommendations