Advertisement

Implicit bias and social schema: a transactive memory approach

  • Valerie SoonEmail author
Article

Abstract

To what extent should we focus on implicit bias in order to eradicate persistent social injustice? Structural prioritizers argue that we should focus less on individual minds than on unjust social structures, while equal prioritizers think that both are equally important. This article introduces the framework of transactive memory into the debate to defend the equal priority view. The transactive memory framework helps us see how structure can emerge from individual interactions as an irreducibly social product. If this is right, then debiasing interventions are structural interventions. One upshot is that the utility of the individual versus structural distinction is not apparent for the purposes of intervention.

Keywords

Social structure Explanation Philosophy of social science Implicit bias 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Funding was provided by the Graduate School and Department of Philosophy, Duke University. Many thanks to Felipe de Brigard, Bryce Huebner, Wayne Norman, Alexandra Oprea, Carlotta Pavese, Joshua August Skorburg, and various anonymous referees for helpful comments and feedback.

References

  1. Amodio, D. (2009). The social neuroscience of intergroup relations. European Review of Social Psychology, 19(1), 1–54.Google Scholar
  2. Amodio, D., & Devine, P. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652.Google Scholar
  3. Amodio, D., & Ratner, K. (2011). A memory systems model of implicit social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 143–148.Google Scholar
  4. Anderson, E. (2010). The imperative of integration. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Andrews, K., & Biggs, M. (2006). The dynamics of protest diffusion: Movement organizations, social networks, and news media in the 1960 it-Iins. American Sociological Review, 71(5), 752–777.Google Scholar
  6. Augoustinos, M., & Innes, J. (1990). Towards an integration of social representations and social schema theory. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29(3), 213–231.Google Scholar
  7. Ayala, S. (2016). Comments on Madva (2016). In Ergo symposium.Google Scholar
  8. Ayala, S. (2018). A structural explanation of injustice in conversations: It’s about norms. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 99(4), 726–748.Google Scholar
  9. Ayala, S., & Vasileyva, N. (2015). Explaining injustice in speech: Individualistic versus structural explanation. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual conference of the cognitive science society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  10. Banks, R., & Ford, R. (2009). (How) does unconscious bias matter: Law, politics, and racial inequality. Emory Law Journal, 58(5), 1053–1122.Google Scholar
  11. Bicchieri, C., & Funcke, A. (2018). Norm change: Trendsetters and social structure. Social Research, 85(1), 1–21.Google Scholar
  12. Bishop, B. (2009). The big sort: Why the clustering of like-minded America is tearing us apart. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
  13. Blum, L. (2002). Racism: What it is and what it isn’t. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 21(3), 203–218.Google Scholar
  14. Bradshaw, S., & Howard, P. (2018). Challenging truth and trust: A global inventory of organized social media manipulation. Working Paper Oxford, UK: Project on Computational Propaganda. comprop.oii.ox.ac.uk.Google Scholar
  15. Bratman, M. E. (1993). Shared intention. Ethics, 104(1), 97–113.Google Scholar
  16. CBS News. (2017). More than 12M “Me Too” Facebook comments, posts, reactions in 24 hours. https://www.cbsnews.com/news/metoo-more-than-12-million-facebook-posts-comments-reactions-24-hours/. October 17 2017.
  17. Cholbi, M., & Madva, A. (2018). Black lives matter and the call for death penalty abolition. Ethics, 128, 517–544.Google Scholar
  18. Davidson, L., & Kelly, D. (2018). Minding the gap: Bias, soft structures, and the double life of social norms. Journal of Applied Philosophy.  https://doi.org/10.1111/japp.12351 Google Scholar
  19. Del Pinal, G., & Spaulding, S. (2018). Conceptual centrality and implicit bias. Mind and Language, 33(1), 95–111.Google Scholar
  20. Doris, J. (2015). Talking to ourselves: Reflection, ignorance and agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Elster, J. (1982). The case for methodological individualism. Theory and Society, 11(4), 453–482.Google Scholar
  22. Elster, J. (2007). Explaining social behavior: More nuts and bolts for the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Fazio, R. (2007). Attitudes as object—evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 603–637.Google Scholar
  24. Garcia, J. L. (1996). The heart of racism. Journal of Social Philosophy, 27(1), 5–46.Google Scholar
  25. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692.Google Scholar
  26. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative–propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 59–127). Academic Press.  https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00002-0
  27. Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2007). What do implicit measures tell us?: Scrutinizing the validity of three common assumptions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(2), 181–193.Google Scholar
  28. Ghosh, V., & Gilboa, A. (2014). What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia, 53, 104–114.Google Scholar
  29. Greenwald, A. G., & Hamilton Krieger, L. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94(4), 945–967.Google Scholar
  30. Hahn, A. & Gawronski, B. (2018). Facing one’s implicit biases: From awareness to acknowledgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.Google Scholar
  31. Haslanger, S. (2012). Resisting reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Haslanger, S. (2015). Social structure, narrative, and explanation. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 45(1), 1–15.Google Scholar
  33. Haslanger, S. (2016a). What is a (social) structural explanation? Philosophical Studies, 173(1), 113–130.Google Scholar
  34. Haslanger, S. (2016b). Comments on Madva (2016). In Ergo symposium.Google Scholar
  35. He, J., Butler, B., & King, W. R. (2007). Team cognition: Development and evolution in software project teams. Journal of Management Information Systems, 24, 261–292.Google Scholar
  36. Hoffman, D., & Tarzian, A. (2001). The girl who cried pain: A bias against women in the treatment of pain. The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 29, 13–27.Google Scholar
  37. Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., & Schmitt, M. (2009). The road to the unconscious self not taken: Discrepancies between self-and observer-inferences about implicit dispositions from nonverbal behavioural cues. European Journal of Personality: Published for the European Association of Personality Psychology, 23(4), 343–366.Google Scholar
  38. Holroyd, J., & Kelly, D. (2016). Implicit bias, character, and control. In A. Masala & J. Webber (Eds.), From personality to virtue: Essays on the philosophy of character. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Huebner, B. (2016a). Implicit bias, reinforcement learning, and scaffolded moral cognition. In M. Brownstein & J. Saul (Eds.), Implicit bias and philosophy: Metaphysics and epistemology (Vol. 1, pp. 47–79). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Huebner, B. (2016b). Transactive memory reconstructed: Rethinking Wegner’s research program. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 54(1), 48–69.Google Scholar
  41. Huet, E. (2016). Rise of the bias busters: How unconscious bias became Silicon Valley’s newest target.” Forbes. https://www.forbes.com/sites/ellenhuet/2015/11/02/rise-of-the-bias-busters-how-unconscious-bias-became-silicon-valleys-newest-target/#46994a0e19b5. November 2, 2016.
  42. Hyun, J. (2009). Breaking the bamboo ceiling: Career strategies for Asians. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  43. Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J., & van Kamp, S. (2007). The impact of counterstereotypic training and related correction processes on the application of stereotypes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(2), 138–156.Google Scholar
  44. Lewis, K. (2004). Knowledge and performance in knowledge-worker teams: A longitudinal study of transactive memory systems. Management Science, 50, 1519–1533.Google Scholar
  45. Lewis, K., & Herndon, B. (2011). Transactive memory systems: Current issues and future research directions. Organization Science, 22(5), 1254–1265.Google Scholar
  46. List, C., & Spiekermann, K. (2013). Methodological individualism and holism in political science: A reconciliation. American Political Science Review, 107(4), 629–643.Google Scholar
  47. Lowery, W. (2016). Aren’t more white people than black people killed by the police? Yes, but no. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/post-nation/wp/2016/07/11/arent-more-white-people-than-black-people-killed-by-police-yes-but-no/. Accessed 25 May 2017.
  48. Machery, E., Faucher, L., & Kelly, D. (2010). On the alleged inadequacies of psychological explanations of racism. The Monist, 93(2), 228–254.Google Scholar
  49. Madva, A. (2016a). A plea for anti-anti-individualism: How oversimple psychology misleads social policy. Ergo, 3(27), 701–728.Google Scholar
  50. Madva, A. (2016b). Replies to commentators. In Ergo symposium.Google Scholar
  51. Madva, A. (2017). Biased against de-biasing: On the role of (institutionally sponsored) self-transformation in the struggle against prejudice. Ergo, 4(6), 145–179.Google Scholar
  52. Mandelbaum, E. (2016). Attitude, inference, association: On the propositional structure of implicit bias. Noûs, 50(3), 629–658.Google Scholar
  53. Moreland, R. L., Argote, L., & Krishnan, R. (1996). Socially shared sognition at work: Rransactive memory and group performance. In J. L. Nye & A. M. Brower (Eds.), What’s social about social cognition? Research on socially shared cognition in small groups (pp. 57–84). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.Google Scholar
  54. North Carolina Commission on Racial and Ethnic Disparities in the Criminal Justice System. (2015). Source: http://ncracialjustice.org/projects/implicit-bias-trainings/. Accessed 25 May 2017.
  55. Nosek, B., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101.Google Scholar
  56. Ohlheiser, A. (2017). The woman behind ‘Me Too’ knew the power of the phrase when she created it—10 years ago. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-intersect/wp/2017/10/19/the-woman-behind-me-too-knew-the-power-of-the-phrase-when-she-created-it-10-years-ago/?utm_term=.982cd5fff088. Accessed 10 December 2018.
  57. Palermos, S. O. (2014). Loops, constitution, and cognitive extension. Cognitive Systems Research, 27, 25–41.Google Scholar
  58. Pettit, P. (1993). The common mind: An essay on psychology, society, and politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Ren, Y., & Argote, L. (2011). Transactive memory systems 1985–2010: An integrative framework of key dimensions, antecedents, and consequences. Academy of Management Annals, 5(1), 189–229.Google Scholar
  60. Skorburg, J. A. (2017). Lessons and new directions for extended cognition from social and personality psychology. Philosophical Psychology, 30(4), 458–480.Google Scholar
  61. Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 465–481.Google Scholar
  62. Trawalter, S., Hoffman, K., & Waytz, A. (2012). Racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. PLoS ONE, 11(3), e48546.Google Scholar
  63. Wegner, D. M. (1987). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 185–208). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  64. Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(6), 923–929.Google Scholar
  65. Wegner, D. M., Giuliano, T., & Hertel, P. T. (1985). Cognitive interdependence in close relationships. In: Ickes W. (Eds.), Compatible and Incompatible Relationships (pp. 253–276). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  66. Wilkinson, E. (2018). United we (don’t) stand: How polarization manifests in sport. In J. A. Skorburg & W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Eds.), Political polarization and morality. Durham: The Kenan Institute for Ethics at Duke University.Google Scholar
  67. Witt, C. (2011). The metaphysics of gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Woodward, J. (2005). Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Duke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations