Hypnotic experience and the autism spectrum disorder. A phenomenological investigation

Article
  • 49 Downloads

Abstract

In recent decades, the focus in autism research progressively expanded. It presently offers extensive material on sensorimotor disturbances as well as on perceptive-cognitive preferences of people with autism. The present article proposes not only a critical interpretation of the common theoretical framework in autism research but also focuses on certain experiences common to some people with autism and which can be appropriately understood by phenomenology. What I will call “hypnotic experiences” in autism are moments in which some individuals withdraw into intense sensorial and perceptive experiences. Following their examples, I use the term “hypnosis” primarily to describe a trance state in which the individuals become highly alert to and awake for an experience of a totally new kind. Through a close analysis of autobiographical writings from people with autism I defend the idea that the particularity of hypnotic experiences in autism consists in a certain qualitative shift within experience itself: what changes, in the hypnotic moments, is the way a person with autism relates to his/her own bodily experiences. If this qualitative shift is indeed difficult to account for within a reifying and intellectualist research perspective, phenomenology offers a large conceptual framework for understanding it. Phenomenology, and precisely, phenomenological psychopathology, will thus emerge as a major device in accounting for such “hypnotic experiences”. The argument mainly draws on the twofold structure of experience which is traditionally used in phenomenological research: it claims that in hypnotic experience people with autism are inclined to focus on non-reified “sensings”, “perceivings” and “movings”, and thus leave aside the object itself and any intentional reification of it. Finally, I will claim that this restriction to mere non-reified sensings might lead to a completely new conception of self and world. In the hypnotic experiences of autism, neither the subject nor the object come to a full-blown and independent existence. A thorough phenomenological analysis of hypnotic experience in autism therefore also has to face the question of a corresponding ontology of these experiences.

Keywords

Phenomenology Autism spectrum disorder Hypnotic Experience Prereflective consciousness Psychopathology World 

References

  1. Association, American Psychiatric. (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV-R) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edition (DSM-V-R). 5th Revised edition. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
  3. Attwood, T. (2003). Understanding and managing circumscribed interests. In M. Prior (Ed.), Learning and behavior problems in asperger syndroms (pp. 126–147). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bettelheim, B. (1972). The empty fortress: Infantile autism and the birth of the self. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  5. Blankenburg, W. (1971). Der Verlust der natürlichen Selbstverständlichkeit: ein Beitrag zur Psychopathologie symptomarmer Schizophrenien. Stuttgart: Enke.Google Scholar
  6. Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1990). Using Obsessions as Reinforcers with and without Mild Reductive Procedures to Decrease Inappropriate Behaviors of Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(5), 527–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Caucal, D., & Brunod, R. (2010). Les aspects sensoriels et moteurs de l’autisme. Mouans-Sartoux: AFD.Google Scholar
  8. De Jaegher, H. (2013). Embodiment and sense-making in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 156–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Foucault, M. (1972). Histoire de la folie à l’âge classique. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  10. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  11. Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8, 465–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fuchs, T., & Stranghellini, G. (Eds) (2013). Editors’ introduction. In One Century of Karl Jaspers’ General Psychopathology (pp. Xiii – XXiii). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation or interaction? In E. Thompson (Ed.), Between ourselves: Second-person issues in the study of consciousness (pp. 83–107). Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  14. Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures, expanded edition: My life with autism. New York: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
  15. Grohmann, T.D.A. (2017). A Phenomenological Account of Sensorimotor Difficulties in Autism: Intentionality, Movement, and Proprioception. Psychopathology, 2017, 50, 408–415.Google Scholar
  16. Happé, F. (1991). The Autobiographical Writings of Three Asperger Syndrome Adults: Problems of Interpretation and Implications for Theory. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and asperger syndrome (pp. 207–242). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Husserl, E. (1950). Husserliana III: Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie und phänomenologischen Philosophie. Erstes Buch: Allgemeine Einführung in die reine Phänomenologie. Den Haag: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  19. Husserl, E. (1952). Husserliana IV: Ideen zu einer reinen Phanomenologie und Phanomenologischen Philosophie, Zweites Buch: Phanomenologische Untersuchungen zur Konstitution. Den Haag: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  20. Husserl, E. (1973). In U. Claesges (Ed.), Husserliana XVI: Ding und Raum: Vorlesungen 1907. Den Haag: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  21. Husserl, E. (1985). In R. Bernet (Ed.), Husserliana X: Texte zur Phänomenologie des inneren Zeitbewusstseins (1893–1917). Hamburg: Meiner.Google Scholar
  22. Husserl, E. (2001). In R. Bernet & D. Lohmar (Eds.), Husserliana XXXIII: Die Bernauer Manuskripte über das Zeitbewusstsein (1917/18). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  23. Jaspers, K. (1965). Allgemeine Psychopathologie. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Klin, A., Mcpartland, J., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Asperger Syndrome. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 88–125). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Klin, A., Danovitch, J. H., Merz, A. B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2007). Circumscribed interests in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: an exploratory study. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kant, I. (2009). In H. F. Klemme (Ed.), Kritik der Urteilskraft. Hamburg: Meiner.Google Scholar
  27. Legrand, D., Grünbaum, T., & Krueger, J. (2009). Dimensions of Bodily Subjectivity. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 279–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lovaas, I., Newsom, C., & Hickmans, C. (1987). Self-stimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 45–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mercier, C., Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (2000). A psychosocial study on restricted interests in high functioning persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Autism, 4, 406–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Merleau-Ponty, M. (2010). Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  31. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). In C. Lefort (Ed.), Le visible et l’invisible. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  32. Mottron, L. (2004). L’autisme, une autre intelligence : Diagnostic, cognition et support des personnes autistes sans déficience intellectuelle. Sprimont: Editions Mardaga.Google Scholar
  33. Mukhopahyay, T. (2000). The mind tree: A miraculous child breaks the silence of autism. New York: Arcade Publishing.Google Scholar
  34. Ortega, F. (2009). The cerebral subject and the challenge of neurodiversity. BioSocieties, 4(4), 425–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Parnas, J., & Henrikksen, M. G. (2013). Subjectivity and Schizophrenia: Another Look at Incomprehensibility and Treatment Nonadherence. Psychopathology, 320–329.Google Scholar
  36. Robledo, J., Donnellan, A. M., & Strandt-Conroy, K. (2012). An exploration of sensory and movement differences from the perspective of individuals with autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neurosciecnes, 6, 107.Google Scholar
  37. Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of Autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  38. Rey-Flaud, H. (2010). L’enfant qui s’est arrêté au seuil du langage : Comprendre l’autisme. Paris: Flammarion.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sass, L., & Parnas, J. (2003). Self-disturbance and schizophrenia: Structure, specificity, pathogenesis (current issues, new directions). Schizophrenia Research, 152(1), 5–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. de Saussure, F. (2016). In C. Bally & A. Sechehaye (Eds.), Cours de Linguistique Générale. Paris: Payot.Google Scholar
  41. Sellin, B. (1993). Ich will kein inmich mehr sein: Botschaften aus einem autistischen Kerker. Köln: Kiwi Taschenbuch.Google Scholar
  42. Schilder, P. (2009). Image du corps. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  43. Summa, M. (2014). Spatio-temporal Intertwining. Husserl’s Transcendental Aesthetic. Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  44. Tammet, D. (2007). Born on a blue day: Inside the extraordinary mind of an autistic savant (Reprint edition). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  45. Tatossian, A. (1979). La phénoménologie des psychoses. Paris: Masson.Google Scholar
  46. Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology and the Sciences of Mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Torres, E. B., Brincker, M., Isenhower, R. W., Yanovich, P., Stigler, K. A., Nurnberger, J. I., Metaxas, D. N., & José, J. V. (2013). Autism: the micro-movement perspective. Frontiers Integrative. Neuroscience, 7, 32.Google Scholar
  48. Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: repetitive behavior in autism: a review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 839–849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tustin, F. (1986). Autistic barriers in neurotic patients. London: Karnac Books.Google Scholar
  50. Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience (6th ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  51. Williams, D. (1992). Nobody nowhere: The remarkable autobiography of an autistic girl. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  52. Williams, D. (1995). Somebody somewhere: breaking free from the world of autism. New York: Broadway Books.Google Scholar
  53. Williams, D. (1998). Autism and sensing. The unlost instinct. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  54. Whyatt, C., & Craig, C. (2013). Sensory-motor problems in Autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zahavi, D. (1999). Self-awareness and Alterity: A Phenomenological Investigation. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. Cambridge, Mass., London: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Fakultät für Geistes- und Kulturwissenschaften, Philosophisches SeminarBergische Universität WuppertalWuppertalGermany

Personalised recommendations