Advertisement

Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 12, Issue 2, pp 253–273 | Cite as

Three dimensions of expertise

  • Harry Collins
Article

Abstract

Psychologists and philosophers tend to treat expertise as a property of special individuals. These are individuals who have devoted much more time than the general population to the acquisition of their specific expertises. They are often said to pass through stages as they move toward becoming experts, for example, passing from an early stage, in which they follow self-conscious rules, to an expert stage in which skills are executed unconsciously. This approach is ‘one-dimensional’. Here, two extra dimensions are added. They are drawn from the programme known as Studies of Expertise and Experience (SEE) and its ‘Periodic Table of Expertises’. SEE, which is sociological, and/or Wittgensteinian, in inspiration, takes expertise to be the property of groups; there are ‘domains’ of expertise. Under SEE, level of expertise grows with embedding in the society of domain experts; the key is the transmission of domain-specific tacit knowledge. Thus, one extra dimension is degree of exposure to tacit knowledge. Under SEE, domains can be big or small so there can be ‘ubiquitous tacit knowledge’, such as natural-language-speaking or other elements of general social behaviour, which belong to every member of a society. The second extra dimension is, therefore, ‘esotericity’. The resulting three-dimensional ‘expertise-space’ can be explored in a number of ways which reveal the narrowness of the analysis and the mistakes that have been made under the one-dimensional model.

Keywords

Expertise Studies of expertise and experience Tacit knowledge Ubiquitous expertise Stage theories 

References

  1. Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance (pp. 21–30). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Collins, H. (2011a). Gravity’s ghost: Scientific discovery in the twenty-first century. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  3. Collins, H. (2011b). Language and practice social studies of science, 41(2), 271–300. doi: 10.1177/0306312711399665.
  4. Collins, H. (2010). Tacit and explicit knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Collins, H. (Ed) (2007). Case studies in expertise and experience: Special issue of studies in history and philosophy of science, 38, 4 [December].Google Scholar
  6. Collins, H. (2004a). Gravity's shadow: The search for gravitational waves. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Collins, H. (2004b). How do you know you've alternated? Social Studies of Science, 34(1), 103–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, H., & Evans, R. (2007). Rethinking Expertise. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Collins, H. Evans, R., & Gorman, M. (2007). Trading zones and interactional expertise. In: Collins 2007 (ed) 657–666, [reprinted in Gorman, M. (Ed) (2010) Trading zones and interactional expertise. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 7–23].Google Scholar
  10. Collins, H. M., & Kusch, M. (1998). The shape of actions: What humans and machines can do. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, Stuart E. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  12. Giles, J. (2006). Sociologist fools physics judges. Nature, 442, 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Grene, Marjorie. (1969). Knowing and Being: Essays by Michael Polanyi. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for the Study of Knowledge Expertise and Science, School of Social SciencesCardiff UniversityCardiffUK

Personalised recommendations