Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 9, Issue 4, pp 503–520

The co-evolution of tools and minds: cognition and material culture in the hominin lineage

Article

Abstract

The structuring of our environment to provide cues and reminders for ourselves is common: We leave notes on the fridge, we have a particular place for our keys where we deposit them, making them easy to find. We alter our world to streamline our cognitive tasks. But how did hominins gain this capacity? What pushed our ancestors to structure their physical environment in ways that buffered thinking and began the process of using the world cognitively? I argue that the capacity to engage in these behaviours is a by-product of increased tool investment and tool curation, which in turn was necessary because of increasingly heterogeneous environments. The minute tools are carried and cared for, they begin to undergo selection for added functions, becoming available as cognitive primers and as signals. I explore the trajectory of this co-evolutionary feedback loop of hominins and their tools, and demonstrate the role tools have in shaping our thinking.

Keywords

Human evolution Extended mind Archaeology Evolution Hominins Handaxes 

References

  1. Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Pearson/A and B.Google Scholar
  2. Cameron, D. W. (2004). Hominid adaptations and extinctions. Sydney: UNSW.Google Scholar
  3. Cameron, D. W., & Groves, C. P. (2004). Bones, stones and molecules: “out of Africa” and human origins. Burlington: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  4. Christensen, W. (2009). Agency in skilled action. Australasian Association of Philosophy. Melbourne: Melbourne University.Google Scholar
  5. Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  6. Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 345–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cleland, C. E. (2002). Methodological and epistemic differences between historical science and experimental science. Philosophy of Science, 69, 474–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2005) Evolutionary psychology: A primer from http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html.
  11. Cowie, F. (1999). What’s within?: Nativism reconsidered. Philosophy of mind series. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Eckhardt, R. B. (2000). Human paleobiology. Cambridge studies in biological and evolutionary anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Foley, R. A. (1991). How many species of hominid should there be? Journal of Human Evolution, 20, 413–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Godfrey-Smith, P. (1996). Complexity and the function of mind in nature. Cambridge studies in philosophy and biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Hawkes, K., & Bird, R. B. (2002). Showing off, handicap signalling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 11(2), 58–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2002). Species-wide manufacture of stick-type tools by New Caledonian Crows. Animal Cognition, 102, 349–353.Google Scholar
  17. Jeffares, B. (2008). Testing times: regularities in the historical sciences. Studies in history and philosophy of science part C: Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences, 39, 469–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jeffares, B. (2010). The evolution of technical competence: economic and strategic thinking. ASCS09: Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the Australasian Society for Cognitive Science. W. Christensen, E. Schier and J. Sutton. Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science, Sydney, Australia.Google Scholar
  19. Jeffares, B., & Sterelny K. (Forthcoming). Evolutionary psychology. The Oxford Handbook to Cognitive Science. E. Margolis, R. Samuels and S. Stich. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Kohn, M. (2000). As we know it: Coming to terms with an evolved mind. London: Granta Books.Google Scholar
  21. Kohn, M., & Mithen, S. J. (1999). Handaxes: Products of sexual selection? Antiquity, 73, 518–526.Google Scholar
  22. Kramer, A. (1993). Human taxonomic diversity in the Pleistocene: Does Homo erectus represent multiple hominid species? American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 91(2), 161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Laland, K., & Brown, G. (2006). Niche construction, human behavior and the adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15, 96–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., et al. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(01), 131–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lycett, S. J., & Cramon-Taubadel, N. (2008). Acheulean variability and hominin dispersals: A model-bound approach. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(3), 553–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lycett, S. J., & Gowlett, J. A. J. (2008). On questions surrounding the Acheulean ‘Tradition’. World Archaeology, 40(3), 295–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Marzke, M. W. (1997). Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 102(1), 91–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Marzke, M. (2002). The psychobiology of the hand. Journal of Human Evolution, 42(3), 359–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Marzke, M. W., Toth, N., et al. (1997). Hard hammer percussion manufacture of tools and early hominid hand morphology. Journal of Human Evolution, 32(4), A11–A12.Google Scholar
  30. McNabb, J., Binyon, F., et al. (2004). The large cutting tools from the South African Acheulean and the question of social traditions. Current Anthropology, 45(5), 653–677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., et al. (1996). Niche construction. The American naturalist, 147(4), 641–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N. et al. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Monographs in population biology; 37. Princeton, NJ: Oxford, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Owen-Smith, N. (1989). Megafaunal extinctions: The conservation message from 11,000 Years B.P. Conservation Biology, 3(4), 405–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pitts, M. W., & Roberts, M. (1997). Fairweather Eden: Life in Britain half a million years ago as revealed by the excavations at Boxgrove. London: Century.Google Scholar
  35. Plummer, T. (2004). Flaked stones and old bones: Biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 125(S39), 118–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schick, K. D., & Toth, N. P. (1993). Making silent stones speak: Human evolution and the dawn of technology. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.Google Scholar
  37. Sharon, G. (2009). Acheulian giant-core technology: A worldwide perspective. Current Anthropology, 50(3), 335–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Skyrms, B. (2004). The stag hunt and the evolution of social structure. Cambridge, UK; New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Blackwell: Malden.Google Scholar
  40. Sterelny, K. (2007). Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London (series B), 362(1480): 719–730.Google Scholar
  41. Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Science. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9174-y.
  42. Stringer, C. (2006). Homo Britannicus: The incredible story of human life in Britain. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  43. Stotz, K. (2010). Human nature and cognitive-developmental niche construction. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Science. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9178-7.
  44. Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36(2), 110–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(03), 299–313.Google Scholar
  46. Sutton, J. (2007). Batting, habit and memory: The embodied mind and the nature of skill. Sport in Society, 10(5), 763–786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tattersall, I. (1995). The fossil trail: How we know what we think we know about human evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Tocheri, M. W., Marzke, M. W., et al. (2003). Functional capabilities of modern and fossil hominid hands: Three-dimensional analysis of trapezia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122, 101–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Vrba, E. S., Denton, G. H., et al. Eds. (1995). Paleoclimate and evolution, with emphasis on human origins. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Whiten, A., Goodall, J., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Whiten, A., Schick, K., et al. (2009). The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology: Integrating evidence from palaeoanthropology and primatology. Journal of Human Evolution, 57(4), 420–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wood, B. A., & Collard, M. (2001). The changing face of the genus homo. Evolutionary Anthropology, 8(6), 195–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 25(3), 389–438.Google Scholar
  54. Young, R. W. (2003). Evolution of the human hand: The role of throwing and clubbing. Journal of Anatomy, 202(1), 165–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Victoria University of WellingtonWellingtonNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations