Advertisement

Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 7, Issue 4, pp 485–500 | Cite as

On being stuck in time

  • Christoph HoerlEmail author
Article

Abstract

It is sometimes claimed that non-human animals (and perhaps also young children) live their lives entirely in the present and are cognitively ‘stuck in time’. Adult humans, by contrast, are said to be able to engage in ‘mental time travel’. One possible way of making sense of this distinction is in terms of the idea that animals and young children cannot engage in tensed thought, which might seem a preposterous idea in the light of certain findings in comparative and developmental psychology. I try to make this idea less preposterous by looking into some of the cognitive requirements for tensed thought. In particular, I suggest that tensed thought requires a specific form of causal understanding, which animals and young children may not possess.

Keywords

Mental time travel Tensed thought Causal understanding 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Many of the lines of thought developed in this paper emerged from discussions I have had over the years with Teresa McCormack. The paper itself started life as my contribution to a symposium on mental time travel that she organized at the 2006 meeting of the European Society for Philosophy and Psychology at Queen’s University Belfast, where the other symposiasts were Cristina Atance and Thomas Zentall. Later versions of the paper were also presented at seminars at Macquarie University, Sydney, and at the Australian National University, Canberra. I am very grateful to the audiences at those different occasions, and to two anonymous referees for this journal, for a variety of helpful comments. Work on this paper took place within the context of the AHRC Project on Causal Understanding.

References

  1. Aristotle (1930). On memory and reminiscence. In W. D. Ross (Ed.) The works of Aristotle (vol. 3). Oxford: Clarendon Press (translated by J. I. Beare; originally written ca. 350).Google Scholar
  2. Atance, C. (2006). Contribution to symposium on mental time travel, Annual meeting of the European Society for Philosophy and Psychology, Belfast, August 2006.Google Scholar
  3. Atance, C. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2006). Preschoolers’ current desires warp their choices for the future. Psychological Science, 17, 583–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J., & Apperly, I. A. (2006). Children’s thinking about counterfactuals and future hypotheticals as possibilities. Child Development, 77, 413–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bennett, J. (1964). Rationality: an essay towards an analysis. London, U.K.: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  6. Bennett, J. (1976). Linguistic behaviour. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bergson, H. (1988). Matter and memory,. Cambridge, MA: Zone Books (trans by N. M. Paul and W. S. Palmer; originally published in 1896).Google Scholar
  8. Campbell, J. (2006). Ordinary thinking about time. In F. Stadler, & M. Stoeltzner (Eds.) Time and history: Proceedings of the 28th international Wittgenstein symposium 2005. Frankfurt: Ontos verlag.Google Scholar
  9. Clayton, N. S., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by Scrub-jays. Nature, 395, 272–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J., Emery, N. J., & Dickinson, A. (2003). Prometheus to Proust: the case for behavioural criteria for ‘mental time travel’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 436–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Davidson, D. (1982). Rational animals. Dialectica, 36, 317–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Kort, S. R., Dickinson, A., & Clayton, N. S. (2005). Retrospective cognition by food-caching western scrub-jays. Learning and Motivation, 36, 159–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dennett, D. C. (2005). Sweet dreams: Philosophical obstacles to a science of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  14. Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Eichenbaum, H., Fortin, N. J., Ergorul, C., Wright, S. P., & Agster, K. L. (2005). Episodic recollection in animals: “If it walks like a duck and quacks like a duck...”. Learning and Motivation, 36, 190–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Griffiths, D. P., Dickinson, A., & Clayton, N. S. (1999). Declarative and episodic memory: what can animals remember about their past? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 74–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Henderson, J., Hurly, T. A., Bateson, M., & Healy, S. D. (2006). Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds Selasphorus rufus. Current Biology, 16, 512–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoerl, C. (2001). The phenomenology of episodic recall. In C. Hoerl, & T. McCormack (Eds.) Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  19. Koehler, W. (1927). The mentality of apes. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul (translated by E. Winter, originally published 1917).Google Scholar
  20. McCormack, T. (2001). Attributing episodic memory to animals and children. In C. Hoerl, & T. McCormack (Eds.) Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  21. McCormack, T., & Hoerl, C. (1999). Memory and temporal perspective: The role of temporal frameworks in memory development. Developmental Review, 19, 154–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McCormack, T., & Hoerl, C. (2001). The child in time: Temporal concepts and self-consciousness in the development of episodic memory. In C. Moore, & K. Lemmon (Eds.) The self in time: developmental perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. McCormack, T., & Hoerl, C. (2007). Young children’s reasoning about the order of past events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 168–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Martin, M. G. F. (2001). Episodic recall as retained acquaintance. In C. Hoerl, & T. McCormack (Eds.) Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  25. Nietzsche, F. (1983). Untimely meditations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (translated by R.J. Hollingdale, originally published 1876).Google Scholar
  26. Peacocke, C. (1999). Being Known. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  27. Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Stöttinger, E. (2007). Introspection and remembering. Synthese, 159, 253–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Povinelli, D. J., Landry, A. M., Theall, L. A., Clark, B. R., & Castille, C. M. (1999). Development of young children’s understanding that the recent past is causally bound to the present. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1426–1439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schopenhauer, A. (1999). Prize essay on the freedom of the will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (edited by G. Zöller, translated by E.F.J. Payne; originally published 1840).Google Scholar
  30. Smith, P. (1982). Bennett’s beliefs. Philosophical Studies, 41, 431–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2003). Mental time travel in animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 391–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36, 110–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.Google Scholar
  34. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–313.Google Scholar
  35. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving, & W. Donaldson (Eds.) Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  36. Tulving, E. (2001). Episodic memory and common sense: How far apart? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 356, 1505–1515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wagner, L. (2001). Aspectual influences on early tense comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 28, 661–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wearden, J. (2001). Internal clocks and the representation of time. In C. Hoerl, & T. McCormack (Eds.) Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  39. Zentall, T. R. (2005). Animals may not be stuck in time. Learning and Motivation, 36, 208–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Zentall, T. R. (2006). Mental time travel in animals: a challenging question. Behavioural Processes, 72, 173–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of WarwickCoventryUK

Personalised recommendations