Advertisement

Neuropsychology Review

, Volume 22, Issue 4, pp 345–360 | Cite as

Assessing Executive Function in Preschoolers

  • Peter J. AndersonEmail author
  • Natalie Reidy
Review

Abstract

Executive function develops at an unprecedented rate during the preschool period, yet few clinicians attempt to assess executive processes in young children. The primary objective of this article is to demonstrate that executive function can be assessed in preschoolers, and to highlight the importance of detecting executive dysfunction as early as possible. Following a description of executive function and the underlying neural systems, this article outlines some of the challenges in assessing executive function in young children. The various assessment paradigms used for assessing executive function in preschoolers are presented, and based on studies that have applied these measurement tools normal development of executive domains is described. Finally, the benefits and opportunities for executive function intervention in the preschool period are considered.

Keywords

Executive function Development Assessment Preschool Children 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Financial support was provided by the National Health & Medical Research Council (PJA - Senior Research Fellowship #628371) and the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program.

References

  1. Alexander, M., & Stuss, D. (2000). Disorders of frontal lobe functioning. Seminars in Neurology, 20(4), 427–437.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71–82.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, P., Doyle, L., & Victorian Infant Collaborative Group. (2004). Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Pediatrics, 114(1), 50–57.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, P. J. (2008). Towards a developmental model of executive function. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 3–22). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  5. Anderson, V. (1998). Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(3), 319–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Anderson, V., Godfrey, C., Rosenfeld, J. V., & Catroppa, C. (2012). 10 years outcome from childhood traumatic brain injury. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(3), 217–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Anderson, V. A., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Mikiewicz, O. (2002). Relationships between cognitive and behavioral measures of executive function in children with brain disease. Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 231–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28.Google Scholar
  9. Baddeley, A. (1998). The central executive: a concept and some misconceptions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 523–526.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: A PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34(6), 515–526.Google Scholar
  12. Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Baron, I. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the child. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Beck, D. M., Schaefer, C., Pang, K., & Carlson, S. M. (2011). Executive function in preschool children: test-retest reliability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(2), 169–193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential Behavioral Effects in Frontal Lobe Disease. Neuropsychologia, 6(1), 53–60.Google Scholar
  16. Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Bigler, E. (1988). Frontal lobe damage and neuropsychological assessment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 279–297.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Blair, C. (2002). School readiness—integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. The American Psychologist, 57(2), 111–127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). Functional neural networks of semantic and syntactic processes in the developing brain. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 19(10), 1609-1623.Google Scholar
  21. Brocki, K., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: a dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(2), 571–593.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). A comparison of performance on the Towers of London and Hanoi in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(4), 743–754.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595–616.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Casey, B.J., Giedd, J.N., & Thomas, K.M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological psychology, 54(1-3), 241–257.Google Scholar
  25. Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(3), 104–110.Google Scholar
  26. Chevalier, N., Sheffield, T. D., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2012). Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: the roles of inhibition and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(2), 99–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Clark, C. A. C., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Courtney, S., Petit, L., Maisong, J., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. (1998). An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science, 279, 1347–1350.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Cripe, L. I. (1996). The ecological validity of executive function testing. In R. J. Sbordone & J. Long (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing (pp. 171–202). England: Gr Press/St Lucie Press, Inc.Google Scholar
  30. Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Spinnler, H., & Trivelli, C. (1998). Frontal lobe functioning in man: the riddle revisited. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(8), 663–682.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development, 56(4), 868–883.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22(3), 271–294.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on piaget’s AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 74(1), 24–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “do as I say, not as I do”. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(4), 315–334.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Diamond, A., Prevor, M., Callender, G., & Druin, D. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU—introduction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(4).Google Scholar
  36. Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 352–362.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9(1), 1–8.Google Scholar
  38. Elliott, C. D. (2007). Differential ability scales second edition: Introductory and technical handbook. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
  39. Eslinger, P., & Damasio, A. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal-lobe ablation—patient EVR. Neurology, 35(12), 1731–1741.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Espy, K. (1997). The shape school: assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 495–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Espy, K. A., & Cwik, M. F. (2004). The development of a trial making test in young children: the TRAILS-P. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18(3), 411–422.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Espy, K. A., Kaufmann, P. M., McDiarmid, M. D., & Glisky, M. L. (1999). Executive functioning in preschool children: performance on A-not-B and other delayed response format tasks. Brain and Cognition, 41(2), 178–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Espy, K. A., Kaufmann, P. M., & Glisky, M. L. (2001). New procedures to assess executive functions in preschool children. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(1), 46–58.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 465–486.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Espy, K. A., Bull, R., Martin, J., & Stroup, W. (2006). Measuring the development, of executive control with the shape school. Psychological Assessment, 18(4), 373–381.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Espy, K. A., Bull, R., Kaiser, H., Martin, J., & Banet, M. (2008). Methodological and conceptual issues in understanding the development of executive control in the preschool period. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifetime perspective (pp. 105–122). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  47. Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A. C., & Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(1), 33–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Schlagger, B.L., & Petersen, S. E. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a "local to distributed" organization. PLoS computational biology, 5(5), e1000381.Google Scholar
  49. Fletcher, J. M., & Taylor, H. G. (1984). Neuropsychological approaches to children: towards a developmental neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6(1), 39–56.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Gao, W., Lin, W., Chen, Y., Gerig, G., Smith, J. K., Jewells, V., & Gilmore, J. H. (2009). Temporal and spatial development of axonal maturation and myelination of white matter in the developing brain. American journal of neuroradiology, 30(2), 290–296.Google Scholar
  52. Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1/2–7 years old on a stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A.C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863.Google Scholar
  54. Gioia, G., Isquith, P., & Guy, S. (2001). Assessment of executive functions in children with neurological impairment. In R. Simeonsson & S. Rosenthal (Eds.), Psychological and developmental assessment: Children with disabilities and chronic conditions (pp. 317–356). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  55. Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). The behavior rating inventory of executive function. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  56. Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent, T.F., Herman, D.H., Clasen, L.S., Toga, A.W., Rapoport, J.L., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 8174–8179.Google Scholar
  57. Golden, C. (1981). The Luria-Nebraska children’s battery: Theory and formulation. In G. Hynd & J. Obrzut (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of the school-aged child. (pp. 277–302). New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
  58. Grattan, L., & Eslinger, P. J. (1991). Frontal lobe damage in children and adults: a comparative review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(3), 283–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Hermoye, L., Saint-Martin, C., Cosnard, G., Lee, S. K., Kim, J., Nassogne, M. C., Menton, R., Clapuyt, P., Donohue, P.K., Hua, K., Wakana, S., Jiang, H., van Zijl, P.C. & Mori, S. (2006). Pediatric diffusion tensor imaging: normal database and observation of the white matter maturation in early childhood. NeuroImage, 29(2), 493–504.Google Scholar
  60. Heyder, K., Suchan, B., & Daum, I. (2004). Cortico-subcortical contributions to executive control. Acta Psychologica, 115(2–3), 271–289.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617–644.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Hrabok, M., Kerns, K. A., & Muller, U. (2007). The vigilance, orienting, and executive attention networks in 4-year-old children. Child Neuropsychology, 13(5), 408–421.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Hughes, C. (2011). Changes and challenges in 20 years of research into the development of executive functions. Infant and Child Development, 20(3), 251–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Hughes, C. H., & Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 35–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2009). Tracking executive function across the transition to school: a latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(1), 20–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28(6), 517–527.Google Scholar
  69. Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. The Journal of comparative neurology, 387(2), 167–178.Google Scholar
  70. Isquith, P. K., Gioia, G. A., & Espy, K. A. (2004). Executive function in preschool children: examination through everyday behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 403–422.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2(7), 475–483.Google Scholar
  72. Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman assessment battery for children-second edition (K-ABC-II). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  73. Kelly, T. (2000). The development of executive function in school-aged children. Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 1, 38–55.Google Scholar
  74. Kiselev, S., Espy, K. A., & Sheffield, T. (2009). Age-related differences in reaction time task performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(2), 150–166.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Harlan, E. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–232.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  78. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY-II second edition. Clinical and interpretive manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio: PsychCorp.Google Scholar
  79. Lehto, J., Juujarvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Lemmon, K., & Moore, C. (2007). The development of prudence in the face of varying future rewards. Developmental Science, 10(4), 502–511.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, 30(6), 718–729.Google Scholar
  82. Levin, H., Culhane, K., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A., Harward, H., et al. (1991). Developmental-changes in performance on tests of purported frontal-lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(3), 377–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Levine, B., Stuss, D., Milberg, W., Alexander, M., Schwartz, M., & MacDonald, R. (1998). The effects of focal and diffuse brain damage on strategy application: evidence from focal lesions, traumatic brain injury and normal aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(3), 247–264.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Lezak, M. (1982). The problem of assessing executive functions. International Journal of Psychology, 17(2-3), 281–297.Google Scholar
  85. Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Bigler, E., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  86. Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia, 36(3), 273–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Matsuzawa, J., Matsui, M., Konishi, T., Noguchi, K., Gur, R. C., Bilker, W., & Miyawaki, T. (2001). Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray and white matter in healthy infants and children. Cerebral cortex, 11(4), 335–342.Google Scholar
  88. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Morris, R. G., Ahmed, S., Syed, G. M., & Toone, B. K. (1993). Neural correlates of planning ability - frontal-lobe activation during the tower-of-london-test. Neuropsychologia, 31(12), 1367–1378.Google Scholar
  90. Montgomery, D. E., & Koeltzow, T. E. (2010). A review of the day-night task: the stroop paradigm and interference control in young children. Developmental Review, 30(3), 308–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Muller, U., Kerns, K. A., & Konkin, K. (2012). Test-retest reliability and practice effects of executive function tasks in preschool children. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(2), 271–287.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. E. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 4, pp. 1–14). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  93. O’Donnell, J., Macgregor, L., Dabrowski, J., Oestreicher, J., & Romero, J. (1994). Construct-validity of neuropsychological tests of conceptual and attentional abilities. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50(4), 596–600.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals - relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Parkin, A. J. (1998). The central executive does not exist. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 518–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., & Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain research bulletin, 54(3), 255–266.Google Scholar
  97. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Prevor, M. B., & Diamond, A. (2005). Color-object interference in young children: a stroop effect in children 3(1/2)-6(1/2) years old. Cognitive Development, 20(2), 256–278.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Rezai, K., Andreasen, N., Alliger, R., Cohen, G., Swayze, V., & O’Leary, D. (1993). The neuropsychology of the prefrontal cortex. Archives of Neurology, 50, 636–642.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., & Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 638–662.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-binet intelligence scales, fifth edition: Technical manual. Itasca: Riverside.Google Scholar
  102. Rueda, M., Rothbart, M., McCandliss, B., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(41), 14931–14936.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Schoemaker, K., Bunte, T., Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., Dekovic, M., & Matthys, W. (2012). Executive function deficits in preschool children with ADHD and DBD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53(2), 111–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Shallice, T. (1990). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  106. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organisation of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 351, 1405–1412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Smidts, D. P., Jacobs, R., & Anderson, V. (2004). The object classification task for children (OCTC): a measure of concept generation and mental flexibility in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 385–401.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223–8231.Google Scholar
  109. Stuss, D. T. (1992). Biological and psychological-development of executive functions. Brain and cognition, 20(1), 8–23.Google Scholar
  110. Stuss, D., & Alexander, M. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 289–298.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Floden, D., Binns, M. A., Levine, B., McIntosh, A. R., et al. (2002). Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal lesions in humans. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Thyreau, B., Sassa, Y., Takeuchi, H., Wu, K., Kotozaki, Y., Nouchi, R., Asano, M., Asano, K., Fukuda, H., & Kawashima, R. (2012). Linear and curvilinear correlations of brain gray matter volume and density with age using voxel-based morphometry with the Akaike information criterion in 291 healthy children. Human brain mapping. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22033.
  113. Tau, G. Z., & Peterson, B. S. (2010). Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 147–168.Google Scholar
  114. Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Tsujimoto, S. (2008). The prefrontal cortex: functional neural development during early childhood. The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry, 14(4), 345–358.Google Scholar
  116. Tsujimoto, S., Yamamoto, T., Kawaguchi, H., Koizumi, H., & Sawaguchi, T. (2004). Prefrontal cortical activation associated with working memory in adults and preschool children: an event-related optical topography study. Cerebral cortex, 14(7), 703–712.Google Scholar
  117. Walsh, K. (1978). Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. New York: Churchill Livingston.Google Scholar
  118. Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence (3rd ed.). San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
  119. Welsh, M., & Pennington, B. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: views from developmental psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(3), 199–230.Google Scholar
  120. Welsh, M., Pennington, B., Ozonoff, S., Rouse, B., & McCabe, E. (1990). Neuropsychology of early-treated phenylketonuria - specific executive function deficits. Child Development, 61(6), 1697–1713.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Welsh, M., Pennington, B., & Groisser, D. (1991). A normative developmental-study of executive function—a window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(2), 131–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: 1. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 575–587.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Willoughby, M., & Blair, C. (2011). Test-retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 17(6), 564–579.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., & Greenberg, M. (2010). The measurement of executive function at age 3 years: psychometric properties and criterion validity of a new battery of tasks. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 306–317.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., & Blair, C. B. (2011). Contributions of modern measurement theory to measuring executive function in early childhood: an empirical demonstration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 414–435.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental psychobiology, 44(1), 68–83.Google Scholar
  127. Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297–301.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3).Google Scholar
  129. Zelazo, P., Qu, L., & Muller, U. (2004). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: relations in early development. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71–93). Mahwah: Lawrence Eribaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Victorian Infant Brain Studies (VIBeS)Murdoch Childrens Research InstituteParkvilleAustralia
  2. 2.Department of PaediatricsThe University of MelbourneMelbourneAustralia

Personalised recommendations